Website Sections
- Home Page
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Transhuman News Blog
- Prometheism Religion of Transhumanism
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
The Psychology of anti-White hate
Since the Left uses racism as the battering ram to enforce an egalitarian agenda to redistribute resources from Whites to other "people of color," hate is a topical subject that has not received enough attention in terms of empirical data. For example, research on altruism and sexual selectionto name just twocan fill volumes. But hate, has not been studied as intensely as the other areas of human emotions, so when I saw the book The Psychology of Hate, edited by Robert J. Sternberg, 2004, it was a must read.
First, any study of hate is inclusive of disgust, fear, and anger and should include the broad categories of ethnocentrism and its other sidexenophobia. As such, studying hate cannot be undertaken without also including the genetic component that has evolved and why and in what ways humans hate as a means of improving individual and especially group fitness within the study of group evolutionary strategies.
Unfortunately, this book failed miserably as an empirical attempt to understand hate, and instead leaned heavily towards simplistic formulations to try and eliminate hate by merely describing it, and implicating Whites most often in its cause and effect on others. I really didn't expect anything else, but occasionally surprises do happen. I know Robert J. Sternberg's work, and he comes from the Marxist wing of the deniers of racial differences. With Sternberg as editor, I suspected this book would be biased, and I was correct. So the best I can do, rather than shed any more light on the evolutionary basis of hate, anger, fear and disgust, is to shed some light on how even today's psychology is very close to the Standard Social Science Model (SSSM), and therefore unable to produce good research.
I am reading the book Adapting Minds: Evolutionary Psychology and the
Persistent Quest for Human Nature, by David J. Buller, 2005, and he states,
"But Gould's argument fails to substantiate even this weaker charge. For Evolutionary Psychologists answer Gould's question
by claiming that there are three
sources from which we can obtain information about the [Environment of
Evolutionary Adaptation] EEA: the design of our adaptations, studies of
extant hunter-gatherer societies, and primate studies. Therefore,
showing that we can't possibly have any evidence for Evolutionary Psychology's
adaptive hypotheses requires more than Gould's rhetorical question about how we
can know what our ancestors did two million years ago; it requires examining
the three sources of evidence that Evolutionary Psychologists claim can substantiate
their adaptive hypotheses. Gould, however, fails to discuss these sources of
evidence and their relevance to his argument."
Likewise, as the introduction to The Psychology of Hate
admits, "Psychologists have not generated a lot of theories of hate,
certainly fewer than theories of love. A survey of some recent introductory
social psychology texts revealed love
as an index term in all of them but hate as an index term in none of them. The goal of this book
is to help redress an imbalanceto propose a number of different theories that
answer questions about hate in related, but different, ways. The theories
proposed in this book cover the gamut,
including clinical, cognitive, social, and eclectic emphases on understanding
hate.
"Authors have been
asked to address a common set of questions to ensure unity of their
contributions: How do you conceptualize hate?; What evidence is there for this
conceptualization?; How does your view relate to other views?; What do you see
as the role of hate in terrorism, massacres, and genocides?; How, if at all,
can hate be assessed?; How, if at all, can hate be combated?"
Note how they did not
include "is there a genetic component to hate," or "how do we
know that hate was not just as important as love in human evolution, and
continues today as a mechanism for protecting a group's or an individual's self
interests and resource acquisition?" No, the agenda was a more dialectical
one, one conveniently used to establish fighting hate as a political tool for
furthering an egalitarian agenda. That is Sternberg's style, set out a
political agenda, then use pseudoscience to support it.
In Chapter 1, "
The above hardly
seems useful for slicing and dicing types of hate. Hate, anger, disgust and
fear actually can be divided by two very different contexts from our
evolutionary past: one is anger, fear and dominance within tribal units and
today's ethnic or social groups as individuals compete. The second is fear and
disgust along with group commitment to act against another group. The context
therefore between individuals and between groups brings forth very different
behavioral traits in people. This book ignores this well researched
distinction.
Edward B. Royzman,
Clark McCauley, and Paul Rozin again, "The theme of hate as the result of
being helplessly 'reduced in one's personhood' is also prominent in the work of
McKellar. McKellar identified two factors that appeared to be 'favorable to the
development of hostile attitudes,' including hate. The first factor concerned the
association between hate and 'unexpressed hostility.' The second factor
concerned the nature of the negative experiences that trigger such hostile
responses. Personal humiliation was the single largest category of such
experiences. 'Physical pain' and 'threat to values' were tied for the second
place, and 'physical pain to another person' came in last."
And later, "In
this sense, the findings that tie humiliation (physical pain or threat to
values) with unexpressed hostility may be explicated as a two-part dynamic:
First, humiliation, physical abuse, and the like are the sort of things that people
of superior status (strength, dominance, rank, wealth) are far more likely
to do to those below them than vice versa. Second, precisely on account of
their inferior status or power, the recipients of this abuse (or perceived
abuse) are likely to inhibit what they think to be a legitimate urge to defy
the higher-status person, while experiencing the physical and mental
preparations that otherwise fit the lay script of anger. Thus, the less
powerful person is both more likely to be abused and less likely to find it
expedient to defy the abuser, making such a person more prone to encounter the
maltreatment scenarios that match the lay prototype of hate. In this view, hate
is primarily a bottom-up phenomenon, a poor man's anger, and as such it is
likely to be shrouded in secrecy. This formulation of the lay meaning of hate
prompts at least two reflections. 1. Both hate and love are likely to be
associated with a perception or attribution of a negative or positive essence.
Briefly, the idea of essence is the hidden something that makes a living thing
what it is. Essence is a more primitive idea than genetics and is better
represented as nature or spirit than as a
biological concept. 2. The negative or positive evaluation of the target of
hate or love is likely to be linked with a moral judgment. The evaluation may
be the product of such a judgment or, as in the case of hate, rooted in envious
admiration; the moral judgment may emerge as a rationalization for the
pre-existing pattern of (direct or inverse) caring. 3. Loves and hates may vary
in the extent to which they exclude any possibility of compassion or ill
wishing, respectively. The implication is that patriotism, nationalism, and
ethnic group identification are particularly extreme expressions of group love,
as genocide may be the ultimate expression of group hate."
This attribution of
hate with power relationships is highly speculative and incomplete. At work for
instance, people do have to suffer certain amounts of humiliation or abuse from
supervisors, but almost everyone has a supervisor and often supervises others,
so no single layer is immune from abuse. It all depends on the individual
personalities and interactions that are not universal.
But power
relationships that include physical and mental abuse are endemic in every
culture and at every level in a social system: short people, ugly people,
Blacks, Whites, homosexuals, etc. can be either on the receiving end or the
delivering end of these power relationships. Blacks for example are far more
likely to attack Whites than Whites attacking Blacks, and Blacks are at least
just as likely as Whites in trying to oppress 'the other' racial group in favor
of their own. Racial politics is all about taking a superior moral stance
against a person or group that can be shook-down by the likes of Jesse Jackson
or Al Sharpton. I am not aware of any reciprocal relationship by Whites against
Blacks in the
An observation made above also seems to be lost throughout the books other contributors: "In this view, hate is primarily a bottom-up phenomenon, a poor man's anger, and as such it is likely to be shrouded in secrecy." If this is literally true, then Blacks are seething with hate against Whites, and this hate is exacerbated as long as Blacks believe it is White racism keeping them down, rather than their own innate abilities. Using this logic, hatred between Whites and Blacks could be greatly reduced if both Blacks and Whites understood the real causes of BlackWhite inequality which means continuing with research on environmental versus genetic differences underpinning BlackWhite disparities, or for that matter WhiteJewish disparities that leads to antisemitism.
Royzman, McCauley,
and Rozin state, "The fourth possible interpretation of a statement such
as 'Hate is a self-destructive impulse turned outwards' is that it represents a
causal explanationthat is, an attempt to specify a mental mechanism most
directly responsible for certain paradigmatic instances of 'hate-related'
behavior. The general idea is that insofar as such paradigmatic instances
indicate the immediate effects of hate, working backward to their (relatively
proximate) causes should give us a glimpse of hate itself. As noted earlier,
the approach to defining hate through paradigmatic instances is common in
political science. One context within which hate is commonly discussed and
thought to be especially problematic is that of ethnic conflict. In this
context, 'Hate is a self-destructive impulse turned outwards' would represent
an explanatory claim concerning the psychological underpinnings of ethnic
conflict; as such, it could be tested empirically by asking if those engaged in
ethnopolitical violence are, indeed, driven by a (sublimated) self-destructive
impulse."
Notice the attempt
to frame ethnic hatred as "self-destructive" rather than trying to
understand its causes. If ethnic hatred, leading to genocide, was
self-destructive then it would not have lasted as a human adaptation to
external threat. Genocide evolved as a means of tribal survival, so it should
be studied as a means of acting out of defense of the tribe. The Left of course
is all about setting up egalitarian norms, and then pathologizing all
deviationsas they define themin order to correct any undesired behavior or
thought. Homosexuality is not pathologized any longer even though it is not the
norm, because it is now embraced as diversity. Diversity in attitudes towards
others is of course no longer to be tolerated by the modern day academic
fascists. (Academic fascist designates the academic Left's intolerance
of scientific openness, and requiring that only certain paths of inquiry be
tolerated, while others are condemned and disciplined by censorship, ad hominem
attacks, and/or defining the scientific paradigm as one devoted to only
egalitarian ends rather than purely scientific ends. It is the rebirth of
Soviet Lysenkoism where genetics was banned and only nave environmentalism
pursued as worthy of scientific study.)
They continue,
"As we hinted earlier, one potential problem with the ostensive approach
to defining hate is that the psychological underpinnings of the relevant
paradigmatic cases may be shown to be very different from one's initial
intuitions about what hate is or is not. For example, many so-called hate
crimes seem to be committed out of some combination of boredom and a desire to
show off before one's group. Indeed, in analyzing Boston police hate crime
files, McDevitt, Levin, and Bennett concluded that the majority (66%) of the
perpetrators were motivated by a desire to escape boredom and get some quick
thrills and bragging rights, with targets being selected because they were
perceived as 'somehow different.' Of the remaining 34%, 25% seemed motivated by
an anxiety-laden desire to protect their neighborhood and families from what
the perpetrators perceived as the onslaught of dangerous outsiders, with the
criminal behavior being seen as a form of self-defense instrumental to
'convincing' the victims to relocate elsewhere as well as forestalling future
'intrusions.' The
This single
paragraph, as important as it is showing that hate crimes are not at all what
they are cracked up to be, is just ignored by other contributors in the book.
Just amazing.
"To give
another example, Gaylin proposed that 'true hate' is a form of mental disorder
('Hatred is a severe psychological disorder'), and he cited terrorist violence
as one of the paradigmatic cases in which such true hate may be found in
abundance. In fact, Gaylin appeared to believe that anything short of portraying
the terrorists as psychologically disturbed is morally irresponsible'When we
assume that at times we feel like a terrorist, we grant the terrorists a
normalcy that trivializes a condition that threatens the civilized world.' We
fully agree that terrorism is evil and presents a tremendous threat. However,
some of the most systematic research into the psychology of terrorism,
including the detailed German studies of the Baader-Meinhof Gang, have found
psychiatric disorder no more common for terrorists than for the general
population that the terrorists emerge from. Again, it seems that something has
to give. Gaylin and those who favor his psychoanalytic approach should either
surrender the notion that terrorism is a paradigm case of hate or be prepared to
revise the concept of hate itself. First, Gaylin does not appear to be
venturing anything like a (falsifiable) causal hypothesis concerning the
motivational underpinnings of a certain type of nominally hate-associated
behavior."
Most of the
literature with regards to racism, hate, authoritarianism, etc., suffers from a
lack of "falsifiable" causal hypotheses and instead relies on just-so
stories about cause and effect. These just-so stories are then recycled and
elaborated upon, while little real research undertaken.
In psychometrics,
the leading method for factoring behavioral traits is the Big Five or OCEAN:
Openness, Conscientiousness, Extroversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. If
any person is evaluated on these five factors, their personalities can be
defined using the five scales. So why doesn't a book like The Psychology of
Hate at least touch upon a supposed behavioral trait profile for haters or
racists or authoritarian type people? Apparently, having primarily a political
agenda and not a scientific one, such obvious questions would get in the way.
They continue,
"Our analysis leaves us uncertain about the much-cited link between hate
and intergroup violence such as genocide, ethnic riots, or hate crimes. If hate
is defined ostensively through paradigm cases of armed conflict and killing,
then the notion that hate is responsible for mass violence is a tautology.
Conversely, if hate is to be spelled out in terms of its lay meaning, as a form
of inhibited defiance, or in terms of a stipulated meaning, for example, as a
syndrome of inverse caring, then the empirical evidence for the link between
hate and intergroup violence remains to be seen. That is, the very status of
hate as a progenitor of evil rests on a prior conceptual decision about which
phenomenon one is willing to probe under the heading of hate and which one will
opt to see as being 'not about hate at all.'
"Most
generally, the four ways of thinking about hate may be helpful in thinking
about many other psychological notions that are rooted in lay experience and
everyday language. Disgust, shame, humility, and pride; frustration and
aggression; value and virtue; the Big Five personality traitsall grow out of
lay meanings. We expect that many such items of folk psychology, and the
empirical literatures surrounding them, can benefit from the analysis
undertaken in this chapter in the service of explicating hate."
So the authors of the first chapter, in making these common sense observations, should have included why the investigation of hate has been relegated to the Standard Social Science Model (SSSM), by ignoring already available research about hate, disgust, fear, and anger from our evolutionary past AND from current situations of individual and group conflict and dominance. Instead, the book operates from a primarily normalized position that "humans should behave this wayand if not we shall pathologize them." It is primarily a prayer book, not a scientific book. Then that is the nature of the debates when science slips into moralizing.
Robert J. Sternberg steps forward in chapter two, "Hate propaganda, which proposes story themes,
typically accomplishes one or more of three functions. A first function is the
negation of intimacy toward the targeted entity (e.g., leader, country, ethnic
group). A second function is the generation of passion. And a third function is
to generate commitment to false beliefs through the implantation of false
presuppositions, the encouragement of people to suspend or distort their
critical thinking processes, and the encouragement of people to reach targeted
(often false) conclusions based on the pseudo-logic of false presuppositions
and flawed critical thinking."
Sternberg then looks
at "hate propaganda" as a process for persuading others to behave in
a certain way. It seems his logic is what is heard every day on talk radio
between Democratic and Republican advocates. The hate is venomous on both
sides, it is devoid of empirical evidence, and it could easily fall into
Sternberg's description of "hate propaganda." It is hard to unravel,
because Sternberg has a habit of defining psychological mechanisms out of whole
cloth, conjuring up new terms and labels, and then pathologizing anyone who
disagrees with him on moral grounds.
As Sternberg defines
his "hate propaganda," foisted upon the unwary by demagogues using
stories, he fails to recognize that this mechanism of indoctrination is used by
every religion, political movement, advocacy group, governments, etc., and for
a host of causes on both the Left and the Right. Therefore, what makes one set
of just-so stories more reliable than any other? When the G8 meet to discuss
international issues, are they promoting hate propaganda, or is it the
protesting anarchists in the streets who are promoting hate? Who is being
hateful and who is not? When it is claimed that I live a better life because I
exploit Blacks in a racist society, is it I who am telling a just-so story
about how it is my talents that brought me to where I am, rather than my so-called
oppression of others? How is Sternberg going to rid the oppressed of their
hatred, when he is unwilling to even consider that some hate may in fact be a
normal reaction for any number of injustices? To complicate things even more,
what does injustice have to do with nature and evolution?
Hate, love, disgust,
and conflict are all part of how humans interact with each other. Commitment
and passion for one's own family is an accepted norm in most societies, and
when they are threatened, hate, disgust, fear, and anger are often legitimate
responses in defense of kith and kin. This is where Sternberg has turned
against science for propaganda.
Sternberg shows his contempt for science: "The risk in work such as this [IQ research] is that public-policy implications may come to be ideologically driven rather than data driven, and to drive the research rather than be driven by the data. Scientists might argue that their work is value free and that they are not responsible for the repugnant or even questionable values or actions of opportunistic leaders.Studying so-called races represents a value judgment because race is a social construction, not a biological concept, and Rushton and Jensen's entire article is based on the false premise of race as having meaning other than in their and other people's imaginations. [S]upposedly 'value-free science' reflects the values of investigators who cannot see their own values underlying their research. In general, when we use a psychological measuring instrument in assessing people, we are imposing a set of values we often do not realize we are imposing. What good is research of the kind done by Rushton and Jensen supposed to achieve? Only vaguely cloaked behind their words is the purported demonstration that certain groups are, on average, genetically inferior to other groups, at least in that aspect of intelligence measured by IQ. I believe that, as in similar past works, none of the claims regarding 'implications for public policy' are justified. As was true of Herrnstein and Murray (1994) and their predecessors, the science risks being used to promote social engineering unsupported by the data. In my response, because of space restrictions, I limit my response to their public-policy claims. The quality of science is determined not only by the quality of problem solving but also by taste in the selection of problems to solve. ("THERE ARE NO PUBLIC-POLICY IMPLICATIONS: A Reply to Rushton and Jensen (2005)" in Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 11, No. 2)
Sternberg then does
not accept the very essence of scientific inquiry, that it is, "'the
cumulative growth of knowledge and power over nature.' That claim does not
depend on any demonstrable increase in prosperity, comfort, or happiness."
(Hayek in Postrel, 1998)
The distinctive
feature of science is that it is the open and uninhibited quest for knowledge
by any means available and for any purpose that one wants to
entertaininterest, playfulness, monetary gains, to be able to out-compete
others, status seeking, etc. The motive for pursuing any avenue of scientific
inquiry is irrelevant because whatever data you present to the scientific
community, there are many more people that will try to refute it. That is,
science, like the free market system, is not a controlled and directed system
for any particular ends. It has only one objective: to test varying hypotheses
against each other by allowing anyone who has the desire to present data and
others to try and refute the data. Hypothesis that withstand this vigorous
struggle between competing theories stand, until they are either overturned or
they are expanded into ever more elegant hypotheses. Nowhere in science is
there room for censorship of any kind, which makes me believe that if there is
truly an authoritarian personality type, Sternberg epitomizes is as vehemently
as the Christian fundamentalists who are promoting "intelligent
design" in order to suppress evolution. They are not trying to promote
science, but to suppress anything in science that overturns biblical authority.
Put another way, "Rauch's concern is protecting
'liberal science,' the process of continually checking intellectual hypotheses
(not just natural science) through decentralized, rigorous, no-holds-barred
discussion. He worries that rules against giving offense threaten that dynamic
process. To work, Rauch argues, liberal science must allow criticism, without
limiting who can participate or what they can say, and it must give no one the
final authority." (Postrel, 1998)
Sternberg thinks that, "Often, people do not create stories, but rather cynical leaders create the stories for them. Often whole governments conspire to create cultures of hate, death, and violence." If that is true, then it is just as likely that governments are now acting to foment hatred against the West and the White people who reside in the West. It also seems from observation that those governments that are the most antiracist to the point of self-destruction, are also the governments in the West where antiracist propaganda is ever present, trying to justify programs of multiculturalism, diversity, and social harmony where it is increasingly evident that competition between racial or ethnic groups will be an ongoing and increasingly problematic social problem for open societies.
The study of hate thenas a collection of emotions including disgust, fear and angermust also look at proximate and ultimate causes. That is, how is hate manifested in the brain (proximate) and how did those mechanisms evolve to become adaptive (ultimate). Hate, fear, disgust and angerjust like sexare part of the old mammalian brain and not the recently evolved analytical brain. These deep and evolutionarily old mechanism are subject to indoctrination, a likewise evolutionary component to tribal life, and they must all be understood together.
Sternberg continues, "As Post suggested, 'hate-mongering demagogues, serving as malignant group therapists to their wounded nations, can provide sense-making explanations for their beleaguered followers, exporting the source of their difficulties to an external target, justifying hatred and mass violence.' These sense-making explanations are what are called stories in this chapter."
Sternberg seems to embrace hate as a conspiratorial phenomenon, one that is completely fabricated by evil demagogues to be spoon fed to the masses for nefarious goals. This tells us nothing about hate itself, but does question Sternberg's own mental stability. Conspiracy theories about how some group or groups are controlling the minds of humans have no basis in reality. Life is far too complex for a few controlling agents to easily conspire to direct the course of history, because there are far too many other actors acting against their attempts at control. It is far more likely, that when it comes to hatred between groups of people, it is a natural outcome of dominance and competition for resources.
For example, Sternberg notes that, "The following techniques seem to be common in
the use of stories to incite hatred and instigate massacres and genocides:
intensive, extensive propaganda; infusion of hatred and its resultants as an
integral and necessary part of societal mores; emphasis on indoctrination of
youngsters in school and through extracurricular groups; importance of obeying
orders; diffusion of responsibility; calls to and rewards for action; threats
and punishment for noncompliance; public examples of compliance and
noncompliance; system of informers to weed out fifth columnists, and; creation
of an authoritarian cult of a leader."
But does Sternberg's
list above apply to hatred between street gangs, or hatred between political
parties, or hatred between soccer thugs? How about hatred between rival cliques
where people work? None of these conflicts have the organization underpinnings
that Sternberg alludes to. There is no central directing hand that manipulates
the masses to hatred. Hatred between groups needs to be understood in terms of
group rivalry and competition, or it can never be understood. Hate didn't just
appear among humans when humans organized themselves 10,000 years ago around
ever increasing social groups. It existed as part of the tribal unit to assure
unity of purpose in the face of danger from other rival human groups. Hate then
is the result of real or perceived threats to values, resources, or revered
worldviews.
Sternberg then gives
us a simple solution to reduce hate, "Building tolerance and creating a
culture of peace and a society in which people share equally in rights and in
participation in the society can go a long way toward resolving problems of violence
and hate. The question is whether people have sufficient good will to achieve
this goal." Again, he offers no evidence that peace will follow from his
advice. We live in one of the most tolerant times history has ever known, with
Western nations passing numerous laws to ensure that minorities have equal
access to and equal participation in the nation's resources and decision
making. Still, Islamic terrorists lash out against Western nations. None of
what Sternberg says makes any sense in light of actual human behavior. He sees
the world through a paranoid's eyes.
Then he shoves one
of his favorite mythologies into the mix, "Ultimately, the best way to
combat hate may be through wisdom. Intelligent people may hate; wise people do
not. People like Mohandas Gandhi, Martin Luther King, Mother Theresa, and
Nelson Mandela had the same human passions as any of us, but in their wisdom,
they moved beyond hate to embrace love and peace. The balance theory of wisdom
defines wisdom as the application of intelligence, creativity, and experience
toward a common good by balancing one's own interests with others' interests
and institutional interests over the long and short terms. By definition, wise
people do not hate others because they care about the individual's (or group's)
well-being as well as their own or that of their group."
Why would anyone
believe that there is such a thing as wisdom as described by Sternberg? An
equally valid description is, "Virtue and wisdom lie in accepting what
nature gives usa life course of
Wisdom, unlike
intelligence, has not been studied nor can it be defined with anywhere near the
accuracy that intelligence can be defined
and measured, or such behavioral traits like introversion or conscientiousness.
Putting forth a theory that "wisdom" can reduce hatred belongs in the
pulpit, not in an academic book that purports to explain and correct the
problem of human hatred. In fact, Sternberg persistently avoids hard empirical
data: "Let the more general lesson of the rising tide of propaganda on
American campuses be clear: If one's beliefs are at increasing odds with the
consensual facts of modern science, the obfuscation, propaganda, threat and
censorship become the principal tools by which one must pursue one's
agenda." (Peter LaFreniere's review of Jonathan Marks' new book What is means to Be 98% Chimpanzee.)
Clearly, academic fascists like Sternberg, Marks,
Feagin, Rose, Kamin,
In chapter three,
"The Origins and Evolution of Hate," Ervin Staub states that,
"Harm doing may be mutual, but
what people see is the harm done to their own group, which gives rise to deep
pain and rage. Out of pain, hurt, anger, and out of the justification of one's
group's actions as right and moral and the other's actions as wrong and
immoral, hate can and often does arise. The dynamics in the group can further
develop devaluation and negative images and through this, as well as through
contagion and other processes, intensify hate. For example, members of
terrorist groups have been described as acting on behalf of both cause and
comrades. They are committed to the cause, but they also are committed to and
want to be loyal to their fellow group members, which intensifies their
commitment to the ideology and their hate for the identified enemy. Moreover,
members of the group striving for leadership take initiatives that move the
group further in the direction they have already started."
Staub's analysis of
hate is much closer to that which is commonly understood by evolutionary
psychologists, and is in direct contradiction to that of Sternberg's. That is,
hate is real and it comes about from real emotions and feelings held by real
people. It is not just a story foisted upon the masses by a demagogue. Soccer
hooligans do not need a leader, nor do terrorists, though leaders may come
forth to help coordinate the group's efforts.
In Chapter 5,
"Roots of Hate, Violence, and Evil," Roy F. Baumeister and David A.
Butz write, "The view of instrumentally aggressive victimization as a
cause of hate leads to two predictions that could be tested empirically. First,
when two people or two groups find themselves in a relationship in which one is
generally the aggressor and the other the victim, the ensuing development of
hate should be more pronounced in the chronic victim than in the chronic
aggressor. That is, if one group regularly uses aggression to exploit another
or appropriate its resources, then the exploiting group may not have to hate
its victims, but the victim group may come to hate its oppressors.
"If victim
groups come to hate their oppressors, this pattern might be expected in the
attitudes of Blacks toward Whites, insofar as Whites have historically
oppressed and exploited Blacks. That is, Blacks may hold more negative
attitudes toward Whites than Whites hold toward Blacks. To be sure, little
empirical work has investigated the attitudes of victim and oppressor groups
toward each other. However, Judd, Park, Ryan, Brauer, and Kraus examined the
perceptions that Whites have of Blacks and the perceptions that Blacks have of
Whites. They found that Blacks judged Whites more stereotypically than they
judged their own in-group. There was little difference between how Whites rated
their in-group and how they rated Blacks. Although Blacks showed patterns of
ethnocentrism, this pattern was not found for Whites."
It is interesting
that they admit that Blacks are more ethnocentric than Whites. In fact, Whites
have been shown to have an unusually low level of ethnocentrism (MacDonald,
1998a). Again, the above seems to contradict Sternberg's conspiracy theory with
regards to group hatred, which is directed from above. On the contrary, it
seems that in the West, we are the only people that have made it a moral
obligation to be tolerant of all other races. I can't think of one non-Western
nation where the political elite has opened its borders, and openly advocates a
multiculturalist and diverse society. This highly unusual moral stance seems to
be uniquely White, but one that is only possible because of indoctrination.
Humans are not naturally universal egalitarians, and therefore they must be
shamed into taking on such a position.
They continue,
"There is other evidence that corroborates the idea that victim groups may
come to hate their oppressors. Branscombe and Wann demonstrated that when a
group identity is threatened, the dominant group that represents a threat to
the minority group is then derogated. Further, Branscombe, Schmitt, and Harvey
showed that attributions to prejudice among Blacks lead to both increased
hostility toward Whites and to greater minority group identification.
Consistent with the idea that Blacks may develop hate toward Whites because
Blacks have historically been the victims of prejudice, Monteith and Spicer
examined essays written by White and Black undergraduates about attitudes
toward the other group. Whereas the essays written by White participants
revealed themes consistent with modern racism (e.g., that Blacks get more than
they deserve), the essays for Black participants revealed themes suggesting
that their negative attitudes toward Whites are rooted in perceived prejudice
and discrimination. Taken together, these findings suggest that people who are
the victims of prejudice may foster hate toward the perpetrators of such
prejudice."
They then go on to
admit that, "Black people commit more hate crimes against Whites relative
to their representation in the population." And note how they have
justified that asymmetry, but accusing Whites of oppressing Blacks, historically. Over the last forty years
however, there has been a large transfer of privileges and wealth from Whites
to Blacks, so why do they still hate us? Because the ruling elite, the media,
and academia says so. They refuse to look at the data that shows that Whites
outperform Blacks because they are, on average, more intelligent than Blacks,
nor do they see the irony that under this scenario, Whites should hate Jews
because Jews are far wealthier on average than Whites. So any group that
outperforms another group should rightfully be hated, and Islamic terrorists
therefore have every right to attack the West because they cannot compete with
the West economically.
It seems from this
logic that group disparities will lead to group conflict, and that is born out
over and over again in modern history. Where different groups have unequal
outcomes in prosperity, and where they occupy the same space, there will be
hatred and its intensity can be fueled by those who blame that disparity on
racism rather than innate ability. So-called antiracists therefore are
perpetuating racial hatred by blaming Whites for all of the hatred in the
world. This blaming Whites pops out of this book over and over again.
They do later admit
that racism may not be the cause of group hatred: "At a basic level,
people who are competing for the same resources are natural enemies. Because
dividing into groups appears to be an automatic part of human nature, it might
also be true that prejudice and disliking of certain groups could be similarly
natural and universal." Aha!
And they continue,
"In summary, the concept of instrumental aggression suggests that acts of
aggression against an out-group can be motivated by a desire to obtain rewards
for the in-group. Hating or disliking an out-group member is not necessarily
part of an instrumental aggressor's motivations, though it may be appealing as
a way to rationalize one's exploitation, and moreover losers in instrumental
conflicts may develop hate toward those they believe have unjustifiably
exploited or oppressed them. From an evolutionary perspective, the need to
procure resources to sustain life or enhance the quality of life may lead to
instrumental aggression if aggressing toward competitors increases the chances
that an aggressor will become the victor."
This is about the
only mention of an evolutionary perspective in this book, and I give Baumeister
and Butz credit for giving the reader a somewhat unbiased accounting of how
humans actually respond to their ecological niches. If peace is desired, then
humans must come to grips with human inequality, at least in ability.
They then go on to
explain that Blacks have higher self-esteem than Whites, and that groups with
high self-esteem tend to be more violent than those with low self-esteem. These
two authors then do embrace, even if it is a timid embrace, some of the
fundamentals of evolutionary psychology. That is, all humans are inclined to
racism, just like all humans are inclined to favor their own families over
others' families. We don't see it at all odd that people often leave their
money to their own children, yet we are currently being asked to share our
resources with other races as if they were kinthey are not.
"Once again,
research on prejudice provides some converging evidence. By most accounts,
overt anti-Black prejudice in the
There is some truth
to what Baumeister and Butz observe, but it needs to be elaborated further.
First, many nave conservatives do seem to think that Blacks suffer from some
moral deficiency, but they are not actively participating in trying to hold Blacks
back or harming them in any way. It merely provides conservatives a reason not
to capitulate to Black's demands for more and more resources to be transferred
to them by making Whites feel guilty. The equally sized number of liberals in
the
"Perhaps
ironically, opposition to prejudice may fuel hate in today's
This refreshing
honesty by Baumeister and Butz could be aimed directly at Sternberg, who fits
the profile perfectly as a person who hates anyone who suggests that there are
races and that races have innate differences. Now we know why the Left is so
hostile to genetic research and psychometricians especially. Hatred for Whites
flows through their ideological agenda. If they cannot blame Whites for the
failure of Blacks to succeed, then they will not be able to position themselves
as being morally superior while disparaging Whites for all kinds of evil. In
the end, antiracists are the most hateful people that one could encounter in
the current world; their target is anything Western and White.
In Chapter 6,
"The Diminution of Hate Through the Promotion of Positive
IndividualContext Relations," Richard M. Lerner, Aida Bilalbegovic
Balsano, Rumeli Banik, and Sophie Naudeau state: "The work of both Adorno
et al. and of McCandless represents a commitment to the idea that hate and the
negative prejudice that may surround it are developmental phenomena. Although
we may not agree with Adorno et al. and McCandless in regard to the
characteristics of the process involved in such development, we agree with
their commitment to understanding the succession of ontogenetic events that
give rise to hate, and thus we support their implicit rejection of an appeal to
the innate presence of the source of hate, for example, to a fanciful 'hate gene' whose influence is, for
instance, estimated through the computation of a heritability
coefficient."
This statement is
quite easy to decipher: these researchers will not entertain any notion that
hate, disgust, fear, and anger have any evolutionarygenetic basis whatsoever.
That is, they dismiss any hypothesis that is not entirely environmental. This
position is pseudoscience since it is well understood that gene-environmental
interactions are the norm for a host of human behaviors. Science comes to a
screeching halt when certain avenues of investigation are sealed off and
isolated, in fear of finding real knowledge over religious fervor. Science has
no place for ideologues that refuse to debate issues from all sides. No one
worldview can prevail if science is to be free from censorship. But they do
state Sternberg's position quite succinctly.
In Chapter 7,
"Hate, Conflict and Moral Exclusion," Susan Opotow writes, "The
psychological literature and popular sources describe the genesis of hate in
humans' sociobiological makeup, in enduring between-group animosities, and in
individuals lacking an integrated sense of personhood. Evolutionary
approaches describe the emergence of hate in conditions that protohumans might
have faced. Informed by studies of animal aggression and human groups in
preindustrial societies, these approaches describe how hate might have been
adaptive as Homo sapiens evolved over the millennia. Intraspecific
intolerance or aggression can benefit a species when it disperses members
and promotes long-term species survival in catastrophes, such as epidemics,
that kill off species members in one locale. However, dispersion results from
other sources than hate, including acute and chronic resource scarcity.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt cautioned that once-adaptive animal behaviors do not invariably
remain useful. If hate was once adaptive for humans, it may not remain so and should
not be viewed as an inevitable product of our evolutionary ancestry."
There are three
misconceptions in Opotow's statement above. First, I will submit that no one
could diagnose what "lacking an integrated sense of personhood"
entails, much less find causes of hate based on it. This is just another
attempt at pathologizing personality typeswhich is strongly rejected by
psychometricians. Humans have different personality profiles that are a result
of both genetics and the environment, and only those individuals with actual
brain malfunctions could be so classified and out of the norm.
In studying
intertribal versus intratribal conflict,
the case has been made that like Chimpanzees, humans fight among themselves for
dominance, but they also form tight coalitions in times of danger that directs
the collective hate outwards towards other tribes. Though the
"dispersal" model of hatred may have been proposed by some
evolutionists, I am not aware of it being as important as the
dominancecoalition ranges of hate, fear, anger and disgust.
Finally, her
argument that hate may once have been adaptive but may not be so today, is no
reason not to study the issue. Scientists cannot adopt a purely constructivists
approach, and ignore totally a more open discussion of an evolutionarygenetic
component towards especially racialtribal hatred. It is interesting that the
clan rivalries throughout the
Continuing to get it
all wrong, "Some scholarly traditions and the popular media describe
deadly, intransigent conflicts as emerging from deeply rooted and ancient
animosities, such as between Hindus and Moslems or Catholics and Protestants.
For example, intergroup conflict in the former
Here, she seems to
embrace the paranoid stance of Sternberg that says hate is directed from the
top in a conspiratorial manner, so that the elite can "control resources
and power." At times, the elite can be the catalyst to foment hatred
between groups, but as I have noted before, in the West, most elites are trying
to suppress conflict between different ethnies. Of course, humans evolved
without "political elites," living in egalitarian hunter-tribal
bands, and to understand the evolved emotions of hate, anger, disgust and fear
we MUST look at our evolutionary past. It simply cannot be ignored. In fact,
her study of Lebanese students fits in very nicely with an evolutionary
explanation: coalitional groups worst fear is encroachment upon their land by
outsiders. This danger mobilizes humans to dehumanize the invaders, to better
attack them and drive them off their land. And it doesn't even have to be a
physical occupation. An outside influence on a peoples culture, language or
politics can all lead to the same hatred toward to outsiders as we are
witnessing in the
In Chapter 8,
"On Hate and its Determinants," Leonard Berkowitz writes, "The
interpretations I have offered in this chapter present a somewhat pessimistic
view of those persons likely to engage in serious anti-minority violence. My
analysis suggests that the inclination to assault (and not only hate)
particular minority groups grows to a considerable degree out of frequent
exposure to decidedly unpleasant situations, particularly conditions that
interfere with the development of a stable and secure self-concept. In
common with other theoretical formulations, I have also proposed that
conditions causing people to view themselves and their in-groups as being on
the lower rungs of a status hierarchy relative to certain out-groups,
especially out-groups they had previously learned to dislike, are among the
major determinants of such a troubled self-image. Although this argument might
perhaps reflect an undue complacency on my part, I wonder if invidious
comparisons of this kind are not inevitable in societies such as our own.
Differences in opportunities, talents, and motivations are bound to result in
some people doing less well than others. Our cultural assumptions and values
tell us that we are individually greatly responsible for what happens to us,
that we are 'masters of our fate, captains of our souls,' so that it is all too
easy to believe it is our own fault that we have not done better in life.
Combining such a self-doubt with the aggressive proclivities arising from other
frequent aversive experiences, some persons are all too likely to blame the
despised minorities for their failures and difficulties."
Interpretation, some White trash will take it
out on Black people when they feel like losers. It is not unusual however for
those on the lower rungs of society to be more violent, as they are often into
drugs, guns, and living dangerously not only due to their often low
intelligence, but because they tend to be "on the street" more. My
wife told me how her father's company picnic of People's Gas employees, always
turned into a brawl. Today, there is little of that outside of some very tough
bars or neighborhoods.
On the flip side
however, those of us who are highly educated and live in diverse cosmopolitan
areas, can also feel a great deal of disgust and hatred for minorities as well
as for a host of other humans with overtly bad behavior. I drive on the
expressways around Chicago, especially those leading to and from the wealthier
suburbs, and invariably the aggressive and dangerous drivers who tail gate,
swerve in and out of traffic lanes to get a few feet in front of other
vehicles, seem to drive German cars. These wealthy assholes elicit the same
hate from me that I direct towards Blacks who stroll across a street, against
the stoplight, holding up traffic, with an overt "fuck-you" attitude.
Normal humans come naturally to hate, disgust, anger and fear, towards others
depending on exposure, personality types, and one's personal niche. It is the
norm, not the exception.
In Chapter 9,
"Genocidal Hatred," David Moshman writes, "Western accounts of
the genocide called up an image of ancient tribal animosities erupting
into uncontrollable mass killings, a prime example, it would seem, of genocidal
hatred causing genocide. On closer examination, however, it turns out that the
hatreds associated with the Rwandan genocide were neither ancient nor tribal. The
Hutu and Tutsi have never been distinct tribes or even ethnic groups; for
centuries before European colonization, they lived interspersed among each
other, speaking the same language, sharing the same religious beliefs,
participating in the same society, intermarrying, and moving across generations
from one category to another, distinguishable only in that the Tutsi, having
traditionally been herdsman, tended to be of a higher socioeconomic status than
the Hutu, who were traditionally cultivators. The sharp and fixed
distinction between Hutu and Tutsi, and the associated genocidal hatreds, were
legacies of 20th-century European colonialism.
"These hatreds,
moreover, were themselves manipulated for political purposes.
This is a good
example of a recent genocidal incident that has been used to place blame for
genocide in general on Western nation's and Western peoplethose evil
colonists. If they had not imposed their racist ideology on those hapless
natives, they would have lived in peace as the noble savages they were meant to
be. The book covers
Note how the story
is spun, "ancient tribal animosities" could just as easily been
"recent tribal animosities." In all likelihood,
Then, there is a
claim that "The Hutu and Tutsi have never been distinct tribes or even
ethnic groups." That is clearly not true as other accounts indicate that
the two groups come from two different races. The Hutu are a sub-Saharan race
and the Tutsi are a mixture of Caucasian-African peoples. This could easily be
shown today by taking a simple cheek swab and having the genes tested. But it
has been shown by the Hutu themselves that the Tutsi were taller, with narrower
noses, and no doubt they dominated over the Hutu, allowing
It could be argued
that the Hutu-Tutsi conflict paralleled the Nazi Holocaust, where Jews and
gentiles lived in peace until a central authority tried to destroy the more
powerful, wealthy, and controlling Jewish minority dominated over
Even in the movie Hotel
In Chapter 10,
"On the Nature of Prejudice," John F. Dovidio, Samuel L. Gaertner,
and Adam R. Pearson write that, "Racial biases are a fundamental form of social
control that support the economic, political, and personal goals of the
majority group. Because of their functionality, racial biases are deeply
embedded in cultural values, such as in widely accepted ideologies that justify
inequality and exploitation and institutional policies and practices. Although
the racial climate in the United States has changed because of shifts in social
norms over the last several decades, racial biases may still be openly
expressed by Whites who strongly adhere to traditional values and conventional
beliefs (authoritarianism) or who see the superior status of Whites relative to
Blacks as legitimate (i.e., Whites high in social dominance orientation).
Moreover, racial biases that are less overtly negative but still function to
reduce threat and maintain the status quo that provides advantages to Whites
are frequently manifested more subtly by many Whites who openly endorse
egalitarian values and who believe they are non-prejudiced. The present chapter
explores the nature of racial attitudes of White Americans toward Blacks and
illustrates the traditional and contemporary role of the psychology of hateits
seeds and its open expressionin race relations."
The above is another just-so story that is pure
propaganda. There is no evidence for a "conspiracy theory" of Whites
getting together to formulate social policy to subjugate Blacks. If there were,
we would not have affirmative action, set-asides, and the incredible transfer
of money from Whites to Blacks to support and egalitarian agenda that is
anti-White. Why would Whites abdicate their own kind for the advancement of
another race?
In addition, don't
Blacks overtly advocate "social control that support the economic,
political, and personal goals of the" Black minority? Is it all right for
one race to a try and advance itself over another, or one species to try and
thrive over all others? Evolutionary models of how organisms behave says it
would be extremely maladaptive to provide resources and support for a competing
race over one's own, and yet that is what these so-called scientists are asking
Whites alone to do. Note, there is not one mention in the book on any other
racial group giving up their advantage, whatever that might be, to advance a
competing group.
They also contradict
themselves. Later on they state, "White racists were more threatened by,
and advocated violence more strongly in response to, interracial marriage and
Blacks moving into the neighborhood than job competition." Really? I
thought we hated Blacks because it was a form of "social control?" I
think they are correct on the above for a reason they fail to understand:
racial groups are often offended by interracial mixing. Racial intermarriage is
loathed by almost every racial group that I am aware of, and the more different
the two groups are the more it is rejected as disgusting. Likewise for Blacks
moving into one's neighborhood; it elicits a feeling of disgust by most Whites
as it pollutes our environment. Blacks are simply seen as less intelligent,
behaviorally aggressive, violent, less controllable, etc. It is no different
than any other aesthetic concern. If races are seen to be different genetically
in substantial ways, it is only common sense that there may be mutual
antagonisms between the two groups.
They continue,
"What distinguishes the different perspectives on contemporary racism are
the conscious beliefs that permit discrimination to be expressed. The aversive
racism framework has assumed that these positive attitudes are based on political
and social liberalism. Symbolic racism theory emphasizes that
beliefs about individualism and meritocracy that become racialized
motivate opposition to policies designed to benefit racial and ethnic
minorities. Modern racism theory similarly proposes that beliefs associated with conservative ideologies
can justify discriminatory behaviors, but this theory places more emphasis on
the moderating effects of contexts that provide a justification for negative
responses to minorities. However, one commonality shared by all of these
approaches, and that reflects the complexity of contemporary racial attitudes,
is the idea that racial bias is expressed in more subtle ways than is
"old-fashioned" racism. In the next section, to illustrate the
dynamics of contemporary prejudice, we examine one of these approaches,
aversive racism theory, in more detail.
"According to
the aversive racism perspective, many people who consciously, explicitly,
and sincerely support egalitarian principles and believe themselves to be
non-prejudiced also harbor negative feelings about Blacks and other
historically disadvantaged groups. These negative feelings can significantly
influence behavior, typically in terms of avoidance or failure to respond
positively rather than in terms of direct hostility. In other words, these feelings, independent
of egalitarian beliefs, may produce negative responses toward Blacks ranging
from avoidance of direct interracial contact to discrimination and interracial
aggression. In the
This diatribe
against Whites is pseudo-scientific, it just goes in circles from one story to
another about why Whites do what they do, feel as they feel, and have a
worldview that may deviate from the authors. The authors in fact must
themselves be extremely authoritarian if they just assume that their
egalitarian worldview is correct, and all others' are lacking legitimacy.
Notice how we are not allowed to entertain a value system that is based on
"individualism and meritocracy!" These academic fascists flat out deny any other value system as being
wrong. And why wouldn't Whites be opposed to "policies designed to benefit
racial and ethnic minorities?" To do so only requires that we favor
policies that benefit our own kind ahead of others, just like families promote
benefits their own children over that of other children. What type of absurd
organism, a thinking one at that, would give preference to unrelated competing
organisms. All one has to ask themselves is "would
Earlier in the book
(Chapter 5) it was pointed out that Blacks stereotyped Whites more than Whites
stereotyped Blacks. So why are all Blacks innocent of racial bias, and only
Whites are singled out for scolding? How do Asians feel about Blacks, or
Hispanics for that matterthere is as much racial animosity if not more between
Blacks and Hispanics. Why are Hispanics not taken to task for aversive racism? Any normal racial group
is going to favor their own, and to try and manipulate other races in order to
take advantage of them when they can. I am not aware of any behavior theory
that states that an organism would favor other unrelated organisms over their
ownnot one instance in the long history of evolutionary science.
It is easy to
recognize anti-White academic programs because the research is always targeted
against Whites. The authors state that, "As predicted by the aversive
racism framework, when the candidate's credentials clearly qualified him or her
for the position (strong qualifications) or when the credentials were clearly
inappropriate (weak qualifications), there was no discrimination against the
Black candidate. However, when the candidate's qualifications for the position
were less obvious and the appropriate decision was more ambiguous (moderate
qualifications), White participants recommended the Black candidate
significantly less often than a White candidate with the exact same
credentials."
My question then is,
how did Blacks respond to the same scenario when they were doing the hiring?
These case studies are worthless on face value because they are always targeted
against Whites. Good research would use the same study, and then compare
different racial groups against each other, in order to obtain unbiased
results. When only Whites are tested for having a genetic interest in their own
race over another's race, nothing is shown except that Whites are acting
normally.
The authors do have
some good advice for how Whites can stop their own self-destruction:
"Nevertheless, despite general norms against prejudice and discrimination,
more local and immediate norms can frequently support racial bias. Blanchard,
Crandall, Brigham, and Vaughn, for instance, found that participants who heard
another student support prejudicial views (which signaled prejudiced contextual
norms) subsequently adopted more racist positions than did those in a control
condition. In contrast, those who heard another student condemn racial
prejudice later advocated less racist positions. More important, these effects
occurred equivalently for participants' private and public responses,
indicating that the communication of these immediate norms relating to
prejudice influenced participants' internal standards."
Apparently then the
latest "social norm" is merely adopted by the majority, no matter if
it is correct, good, truthful, or what have you. Most humans I would contend
are prone to agreeableness, that is they just want to get along and not be
censored. Today, we are in an egalitarian, anti-White, value system where
Whites are made to be the devil. To turn this back all we have to do is stop
going along to get along. Be more open, more honest, and flat out reject any
moral system that says any one race should capitulate to another based on some
convoluted notion of racism as a pathology. Humans everywhere favor their own
kind over others. If they do not, then that is a pathology worth looking
intoand that is one pathology that seems to afflict Whites alone.
"Third, the
forces of in-group favoritism, which represent a critical underpinning of
aversive racism, provide a foundation from which negative stereotypes evolve,
different standards for fairness and justice develop, and members of other
groups become devalued through justifying ideologies. As a consequence, when
prevailing norms against prejudice become weakened or superseded by local norms
that support discrimination, aversive racists may be predisposed to engage in
blatant and aggressive forms of discrimination."
I think the authors
need to look into a mirror, because they are suffering from aversive racism
themselves, if there really is such a thing. What is incorrect in understanding
that group evolutionary strategies will always place one group in opposition to
competing groups, and looking at maladaptation from an evolutionary
perspective. What would be bizarre in terms of evolutionary theory is if Whites
continue to give in to all other races, and stand aside while they are
systematically displaced in their own homeland. That would truly be a first in
the history of all organisms' struggle for survival. It could only occur
because Whites are so easily indoctrinated against themselves because of their
high levels of universal moralism (MacDonald, 1998a).
We then get a
glimpse at the totalitarian solution to White racism: "We have proposed a
variety of techniques for limiting the effects of aversive racism and combating
aversive racism at its roots. These techniques include strengthening
policies and norms against discrimination, making aversive racists aware of
their prejudice and how their biases are expressed, providing aversive racists
opportunities to develop and practice nonprejudiced responses, and altering
the primary basis of social categorization from different racial groups
(i.e., Blacks and Whites) to members of a common super-ordinate group (e.g., on
the basis of university or national identity)."
The Marxists tried
to do that already, and it failed miserably as new groups were sent off to the
gulags based on other criteria than race. Eliminating racism means substituting
individualism and a meritocracy with a new totalitarianism that desires to
control the way people think by passing laws against thinking differently.
Whether it is indoctrination or religious fanaticism, I see no difference
between say radical Islam and antiracists. They are intolerant of any viewpoint
that does not conform to their worldview. What is pathetic however is that
Islam is not fed to us as science, while antiracism is. It permeates our
academics institutions as a fundamentalist doctrine.
And to sum up
enforced group think right out of a totalitarian nightmare: "Revising laws
to combat subtle forms of discrimination can convey an important message to
societyone that would enhance the salience of egalitarian standards and
promote more inclusive social norms." This short sentence is enough to
justify a war on antiracists as being equally as dangerous as Islamic
terrorists. Both are intolerant of free speech, diversity of opinions, and
individual value systems that speak to each person as unique, and not some part
of a unified whole that abhors differences.
In Chapter 11,
"Hate is the Imitation of Love," C. Fred Alford declares:
"Hatred, I argue, is about more than the intolerance of otherness. Hate
reflects a perverted desire to know otherness, fusing with it to become what
otherness knowsor is. In this regard, hatred comes frighteningly close to
love, and love intriguingly close to the pursuit of knowledge, an affinity with
which the West is long familiar (Plato, Symposium). This chapter explores the
implications of this transitivity. If hatred comes close to love, and love
comes close to knowledge, then does hatred come close to knowledge? Yes, hatred
wants to know, but only on its own terms, whose ground rules are utter control.
Although hatred wants to know much, there is one knowledge it cannot abidethat
of its own dread."
This sentence is a
fetishism of Western man's desire to know the unknowable, because obviously
only Whites cannot know what other races know intuitively. Well, ok, maybe I am
not interpreting it correctly, but nonetheless it is a pretty weird statement.
It has no basis in science, and belongs squarely in the conspiracy and
messianic paranoia genre that permeates antiracist rhetoric.
There is nothing
sinister in different races trying to compete with each other, and having
animosity towards each other. Nature is not egalitarian, even if some fanatics
try to equate the two. But why do Whites have such self-loathing for their own
kind, or why do they tolerate other races trashing Whites? That is the question
that needs to be answered if we are going to understand unnatural responses to
ecological imbalances. There are never enough resources to go around satisfying
everyone's greed, no matter what race you belong to. So why single out only
Whites as being evil? What makes hatred of Whites any different than hatred of
capitalists, or Blacks, or Islamic terrorists, or smut peddlers? Are we not all
equal in a desire to pursue what we individually perceive as in our own best
interests? If any person operates to diminish their own well being in favor of
any other organism's well being, that is what should be analyzed and studied as
a true perversion in the struggle for life.
The Psychology of Hate mostly fails in adding knowledge to human
understanding. A couple of the authors made a sincere attempt at scientific
inquiry, but the majority seemed intent on finding Whites culpable for a host
of the World's ills. Diversity of thought, according to these authors, must be
closely monitored and pathologized when necessary to meet an ideological end.
Interestingly, Joan Roughgarden in Evolution's
Rainbow: Diversity, Gender, and Sexuality in Nature and People, 2004,
argues that academia also tries to pathologize sexuality.
Before I look at a
few of her statements on diversity, let me add that her book is a truly
scientific one, and is an exemplary one in terms of scientific exploration of
nature's complexity. However, she does touch on the issues of diversity and
pathologizing groups of people by academic disciplines. Her observations
contrast nicely with those expressed in The
Psychology of Hate.
Roughgarden states,
"This book, then, is a memoir of my travels though the academic
spaces of ecology and evolution,
molecular biology, and anthropology, sociology, and theology. My general
conclusion is that each academic discipline has its own means of discriminating
against diversity. At first I felt that the book's main message would be a
catalogue of diversity that biologically validates divergent expressions of
gender and sexuality. This validating catalogue is indeed important. But as I
reflected on my academic sojourn, I increasingly wondered why we didn't already
know about nature's wonderful diversity in gender and sexuality. I came to see
the book's main message as an indictment of academia for suppressing and
denying diversity. I now conclude that all our academic disciplines should
go back to school, take refresher courses in their own primary data, and emerge
with a reformed, enlarged, and more accurate concept of diversity."
So far so good, she
has accused academics of "closing off" looking at the whole picture
of nature, and suppressing what they don't likein this case diversity in
nature with regards to sexuality. But what about studying the naturalness of
human hostility between groupsincluding racesand giving that branch of study
equal time. After all, most of us identify with a racial classification as well
as a personal view of our gender orientation. Shouldn't science also understand
that it is quite normal for humans, like other species, to favor their own race
over others? Well, she seems to be quite confused in this regards, which I
think sheds light on just how hostile academia is towards a truly open
scientific agenda.
She states,
"The fundamental problem is that our academic disciplines are all rooted
in Western culture, which discriminates against diversity. Each discipline
finds its own justification for this discrimination. This book blows the
whistle on a common pattern of disparaging gender and sexuality variation in
academia and predicts foundational difficulties for each discipline."
If this statement is
true, then it would seem that someone could provide us with evidence that there
are non-Western institutions of learning where there is no discrimination
against diversity. Is that the case? Are universities in
She then notes that,
"More generally, I'm suggesting that females publicly choose mating
partners to manage the genetic relationships of their offspring. Females
guarantee their offspring safety by buying membership in the old genes club and
choose their extra-pair partners with the tacit consent of the pair male.
Females choose not males with supposedly 'great genes,' but males with
well-connected genes. In genetic lingo, females are concerned with genetic
identity by descent, not genetic identity by state. When a female chooses a
male with some special color on his tail, she is not following the dictates of
some inexplicable taste for fashion, but rather endowing her offspring with a
bodily marker of culturally inherited power, like the Tudor nose."
Apparently then the
salience of a sub-specie's genetic markers are seen throughout nature, and race
by lines of descent are important to survival. Roughgarden however decries
pathologizing gender differences, but also seems to condemn Western science
without providing any proof. I am not aware of any other system of scientific
inquiry that is as open as it is in the West. And even in the West, we have
students from all over the world that come here and fit in quite nicely with
regards to an openness of inquiryexcept when it comes to race! Mention racial
differences and the validity of race is denied. Mention that some races are
innately more or less intelligent than another race and the meaningfulness
intelligence is denied or one is accused of racism for discussing the matter.
Roughgarden
continues, "My approach to variation in gender expression and sexuality is
biological and behavioral, not psychological. Since Freud, however, gender and
sexuality have often been discussed in psychological terms. I'm skeptical of
psychology and, as a transgendered woman, have found psychologists to be
dangerous, like gays and lesbians before me did. Psychologists operate with a
medical model that pathologizes diversity. These medical wannabes have long
persecuted and abused gender- and sexuality-variant people from a position of
authority."
True perhaps, but
today most of the pathologizing today is directed at Western White people. We
alone allow our culture and our genes to be attacked over and over again not
only by other races, but by members of our own race who have become so
indoctrinated in the antiracist agenda that their self-loathing overshadows
their genetic interests. And if there is one thing that can be truly
pathologized, in terms of the genetic interests of a species, a race, a family
or an individual, it is behavior that is self-destructive. The science of
evolution is all about fitness and survivalhow have Whites come to this place
that goes against everything that evolution has said should not happen: the
willingness to step aside and be overtaken by one's competitors. The genes for
the Tudor nose do not willingly want to be displaced.
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone