Website Sections
- Home Page
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Transhuman News Blog
- Prometheism Religion of Transhumanism
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
The Affirmative Action Hoax
The Affirmative Action Hoax: Diversity, the
Importance of Character and Other Lies
by Steven Farron, 2005, is a well written book that covers the history of
affirmative action laws, the reasoning that went into how institutions changed,
the scientific underpinnings that are ignored, and how affirmative action
impacts Whites, and a lot more. It is the best book in my opinion that covers
affirmative action and the differences in academic performance between races
based on innate abilities. It deals with affirmative action primarily in higher
education, but the issues discussed are equally applicable to business
set-asides, hiring practices, minority participation, quotas, etc.
Farron begins by noting that, "Usually it is simply assumed [that there are no
differences between races in average mental ability], even though it has
controlled American racial policy for over four decades, is contrary to all
empirical evidence, and a priori
is so improbable as to border on being
impossible. Moynihan mentioned only Whites and Blacks in his study and
attributed the socioeconomic gap between them to 'three centuries of sometimes
unimaginable mistreatment' (Introduction). But from the premise of the innate
equality of all groups of people, he drew the inescapable conclusion (Chapter I,
italics added), 'the distribution of success and failure within one group
[should] be roughly comparable to that within other groups.'"
What is so absurd now, is that affirmative action also applies to East
Asians (who have a higher average intelligence and income than Whites), Asian
Indians who also do well economically, Hispanics even though they may be wealthy
and are very often White, South Asians who are less intelligent than Whites,
while North Africans and Arabs are classified as White, and do not qualify for
affirmative action programs. While many Blacks in the United States are
descendants of slaves, the majority of those who are counted as minorities and
covered by affirmative action programs are recent immigrants. Still, after 40
years of affirmative action programs, economically advantaged Blacks still score
lower than disadvantaged Whites on mental ability exams.
Farron notes that: "26% of Jewish immigrants were illiterate, compared with 1%
of English and Scandinavian immigrants, 3% of Irish, and 5% of German
immigrants. The population density, dirt, noise, and poverty of the immigrant
Jewish neighborhoods had few, if any, parallels in the world.
Nearly half lived in New York, where 'In
1902 a survey found that only 8 percent
of the city's Jewish families . . . had private baths.'By 1919, the proportion
of Jews at elite American colleges was several times the proportion of Jews in
the American population; for example, 20
percent at Brown and Harvard, nearly
25
percent at the University of Pennsylvania, and 40 percent
at Columbia; and these proportions were rising rapidly.
"As I explain in Section F of Appendix
III, when the average intelligence of groups of people differs, the difference
between them becomes more and more pronounced at the extremes. Consequently, the
problem of Jewish overrepresentation was compounded by Jewish
hyper-overrepresentation among the best students. The 1918
meeting of the Association of New England College Deans was concerned largely
with the Jewish problem. Dean Jones of Yale said, 'A few years ago every single
scholarship of any value was won by a Jew. I took it up with the Committee and
said that we could not allow that to go on. We must put a ban on the Jews.' Four
years later, Jones observed that 'despite the handicap of poverty and the
necessity of working their way, the Jews make better average records than their
Gentile fellows.'"
There is a social myth that Jews excel in education because of their social
environments, but of course this is very false. As a persecuted people, they
went through large swings between povertyilliteracy and wealtheducation, while
never losing their innate high intelligence. When given an opportunity to
compete, they outperform every other race of people with a standard deviation
(about 15 IQ points) above the Western average of 100or 25 points above the
world average of about 90.
And yet, the Jews have been at the forefront of promoting the myth of equal
intelligence between racessurely they must know how absurd that is! Likewise,
they have been promoters of using racial bias in educational opportunities
(racial quotas), while they are well aware that similar arguments were used
against them in the past. My personal opinion is that unlike other minorities
who do well compared to other races, Jews have a history of persecution, and are
now trying to promote the idea that Judaism is a religion only, and that there
is no such thing as the Jewish race.
Just like today, to get around innate intelligence, other means had to be
adopted to be able to discriminate against Whites: "Columbia pursued diversity
consistently. In 1945, when its anti-Jewish discrimination came under attack, it
disclosed that it also discriminated in favor of Blacks over White Gentiles.
Columbia showed that the Jewish problem could be solved. Other universities
followed its example by adopting nebulous admissions criteria like character,
leadership, and public spirit, along with the objectively definable criterion of
regional diversity, since Jews were concentrated overwhelmingly in the
Northeast, especially in New York."
In summary, then, the vicissitudes of academia looks like this: Children of the
wealthy and powerful were accepted by universities without consideration for
intelligence. This was seen as inefficient and testing was instituted to select
applicants by ability. The Jews came along, and with higher scores started
displacing Whites. To correct this selection based on testing, other criteria
was added to reduce the number of Jews. Then, after the soviets launched the
first space satellite in 1957, the need for talented scientists made selection
based on intelligence acceptable again and the Jews were back dominating
academiaespecially among the elite. Then along came affirmative action for
Blacks and other minorities, and selection based on standardized tests have been
once again replaced by such criteria as character, race, etc. in the interest of
diversity. Apparently, history doesn't really teach us much at all. Current
struggles for dominance between groups will trump the facts of history and the
best-known facts of science.
Farron notes that, "preference for sons of alumni was instituted for the first
time, as a means of reducing the number of Jews. Between 1927 and 1936, the
proportion of Yale students who were sons of alumni rose from 14 to 30 percent."
Yet this is used today for justifying quotas, without mentioning why this policy
evolvedit was used to select for Whites over Jews just like similar measures
are conjured up today to select for minorities over Whites. Since simple quotas
are not allowed, other factors like diversity, character, etc. are used to
achieve the same results as quotas.
Social economic status (SES) is still being used by most social scientists and
psychologists to escape what is obvious to those in the biological sciencesSES
is a proxy for the genetic intelligence of the parents and thus the average
intelligence of their children. Farron notes, "As will be demonstrated, the SAT
predicts academic performance with great accuracy. The College Entrance
Examination Board added it to achievement tests in order to democratize college
admission by opening it to students who attended high schools that did not offer
a full range of academic courses. I will show that when SAT scores began to be
broken down by race/ethnicity and parental education and income, it was
discovered how democratic they are. We now know that performance on them is not
affected by family, social background, or school attended, because the children
of wealthy, highly educated Black parents, who attend upper class suburban or
private high schools, do worse than the children of poor, uneducated White and
Asian parents, who attend slum schools."
Farron discusses regression to the mean. When intelligent Blacks
marry, their children will regress towards the mean of the population group that
they come from. For example, if two Blacks with IQs of 100 marry, their children
will have IQs somewhere between 100 and 85, or the average intelligence of
Blacks in the United States (85however, there are also regional differences in
average Black intelligence just as there is for Whites). Likewise, when two
Whites with an average IQ of 85 marry, their children will haveon averageIQs
between 85 and 100, the average IQ of Whites being 100. Of course, in every
family the IQs of each child varies greatly because of inheriting different
genes from each parent. Regression to the mean then is more useful when looking
at averages from a large sample of population groups.
Farron discusses why we have come so far from the meritocratic ideals, when he
notes that President Nixon, who was against affirmative action, gave in to
Hispanic pressure groups to have them classified as a separate minority and
given preferences. He notes that many of these anti-White policy changes are
imposed on the majority by minority interest groups. Most of the time, the
public is unaware of these capitulations for political expediency. Since then,
the number of categories for preferences has expanded greatly, with White males
castigated as the cause of all evil in economics, minority advancement, hate
crimes, etc.
Likewise, with American Indians, he discusses how that category has expanded in
numbers because of the benefits received. With a single great grand parent, a
person can join the ranks of the privilegedand many do. Racial classifications
are not always easily visible, so any proof of a Native American ancestor gets
you into the preference club, with all its monetary advantages.
Farron notes: "Every study that has been done of the beneficiaries of academic
affirmative action has found that the large majority are the children of middle
and upper class, well-educated parents. One of the most astonishing revelations
of The Bell Curve was that a Black whose parents are in the upper
one-tenth of the American population in SES (socioeconomic status) is given
preference in college admission over a White with a higher IQ whose parents are
in the lowest tenth of the American population in SES. When a Black from the
top half of American SES and a White from the bottom half attend the
same college, the White has an average IQ 0.58 standard deviation (SD) higher
than the Black. That is the difference between an average IQ and an IQ in the
upper 30 percent of the American population. When a Black and White student from
the lower half of SES both attend the same college, the difference in average IQ
between them is 1.17 of a SD. That is the difference between an average IQ and
an IQ in the upper 13 percent of the population. That is how much superior in IQ
a White applicant has to be than a Black applicant from the same SES to be
judged an equally desirable student." One standard deviation is equivalent to 15
IQ points.
No matter how one looks at the data, the roughly 15 IQ difference between Whites
and Blacks (and the 15 IQ difference between Whites and Jews) has been steady
for over 100 years or more. Since intelligence, when adulthood is reached, is
80% genetic, there is little one can do to change intelligence. The media of
course likes to show improvements in intelligence from intervention programs for
children, but children's brain are still developing and malleable, intelligence
tests just reflect "teaching to the test" among minorities. These gains always
fade away as they grow up.
Farron states: "In 1984, 'the largest and most comprehensive evaluation of the
effectiveness of Title I ever undertaken' found, 'By the time students reached
junior high school there was no evidence of sustained or delayed effects of
Title I [intervention program].' Nevertheless, spending on it kept
increasing. Edward Zigler and Susan Muenchow reported that in 1991 over
$6 billion was spent, even though, 'After pouring billions of dollars into
Chapter 1 for over two decades . . . participating students do not exhibit
meaningful improvements in achievement levels.' The main reason that the
unanimous evidence of the futility of attempts to raise intelligence and improve
academic performance does not affect public policy is that it does not reach the
public. On the contrary, while these attempts are in progress, the media
constantly report that they are spectacularly successful. When their failure
becomes apparent, that is not reported."
Farron then takes on the myth of self-image for Black failure: "Since then [the
publication of John Ogbu's speculations], the media have constantly repeated
this 'Fear-of-Acting-White' explanation for Black academic failure as if it were
a proven fact, even though every study that has ever been done at every school
level has found the opposite. Black students who do well in school are more
popular with their peers than White students who do well; Black and Hispanic
parents value academic achievement more highly than White parents and give their
children more help in school; Black and Hispanic children do more homework than
White children, have a higher regard for their academic ability than White
children, and have higher expectations than White children of future academic
success and the level of education they will complete."
He then goes on to dispel the low self-esteem myth of Blacks. Blacks have
far higher self-esteem than Whites, and that attitude has been constant for a
very long time. It gets even more absurd: "For example, in 1991, a higher
proportion of the students in Washington, DC's public schools answered 'yes' to
the statement 'I am good in mathematics' than the students in any state. But
Washington, DC's students were the worst in the United States in mathematical
ability. North Dakota's students were the best in the United States in math
ability, but the proportion of North Dakota's students who said they were good
at mathematics was less than 40 percent as high as the proportion of students in
Washington, DC. At that time, 96 percent of the students in Washington, DC's
public schools were Black or Hispanic; those in North Dakota were nearly all
White. North Dakota was thirty-eighth among American states in the amount of
money it spent annually per student in its public schools and fortieth in
teacher-student ratio. Washington, DC's public schools had a better
teacher-student ratio and spent more per student than any state, well over twice
as much as North Dakota."
This data is just amazing considering how low school achievement is constantly
attributed to a lack of money provided to minorities' schools. It also shows an
additional absurdity that anyone who deals with Blacks very often should be
aware ofthey will flat out tell you how good they are at almost anything while
the facts are so obviously just the opposite. My opinion is that over the last
40 years or so, there has been so much emphasis on building Black children's
self-esteem, and many of them go to essentially segregated schools, that they
believe they are intelligent. This self assurance has been an ongoing effort in
indoctrinating Blacks"be all you can be" is translated into "you are better at
X than others." As they grow up however, this indoctrination turns into
bitterness and hate as they blame Whites for their failure to achieve their
dreams.
The differences in academic achievement between Blacks and Whites are actually
accentuated. Farron states, "Even among
holders of bachelor's degrees in fields in which the criteria of grading are
nearly completely objectiveengineering, physical science, and mathematicsthe
difference between the average Black and White IQ is 1.1 SD. Differences among
holders of university degrees in other subjects (e.g., social sciences,
education, business) range from 1.4 to 1.6 SD. The average racial difference for
advanced degrees is 1.6. (Two SDs above an average is at the 98th percentile.)
This difference means that the average Black American with a post-baccalaureate
degree has approximately the same IQ as the average White American with no more
than a high school diploma."
The reason that differences are even more pronounced in subjects that are more
subjective, such as social science and education, is that teachers can grade
more subjectively, effectively giving unqualified Blacks a pass no matter how
poorly they perform. And it is only going to get worse: "In January 1999, the
University of Pittsburgh proudly announced that if the average grades (not just
graduation rates) of the Black and Hispanic students in any of its faculties do
not equal the average grades of its White students, the budget of that faculty
would be cut. The spokesman of the American Council of Education praised the
plan because, 'It's clearly designed to put some teeth into campus efforts to
increase educational opportunities.'"
For all practical purposes, grades, degrees, and educational certificates are
increasingly becoming meaningless. "Nevertheless, a student who used a fake
transcript to get into Yale five years later, got a B average for two years
before he was discovered. That was higher than his average at the community
college from which he transferred." So much for the quality of Ivy League
universities.
Farron then discusses the pro-affirmative action in the book The Shape of the
River, that has been used by the media to show that Blacks can succeed when
given a chance in higher education. Farron notes
that the book's authors, Bowen and Bok, were both university deans before
writing the book and, "Stephan and Abigail Thernstrom pointed out, '[I]t must
have occurred to them that it would have been acutely embarrassing if their
evidence had revealed that racially preferential admissions policies had not
achieved their objectives or had produced unanticipated negative consequences.
Critics would have legitimately asked why they had never studied the matter
before. At any time in the many years they spent in charge of two fabulously
wealthy universities, either one could have commissioned a careful analysis . .
. They did not do so, however, and were thus left with the strongest incentive
for giving high marks to a vital part of their own legacy.'"
Bowen and Bok also used university databases with restricted access, and that
access has been denied to others who wanted to review their results. This is
pseudoscience, because science recognizes that others must have access to data
used to independently judge the results by having it reanalyzed. Science is
based on competition between advocates of different theories, and can only be
sustained when the methodologies are open for review by others. Bowen and Bok
closed the door to any reanalysis because other researchers could not get the
same data they had preferentially provided to them by their fellow liberal
advocates of affirmative action.
Diversity of race and ethnicity is in of course, but not diversity of thought or
one's worldview: "Volokh cited a study that found that among the professors at
American law schools, the proportion who identified themselves as Christians, of
all denominations, was half the proportion of all employed Americans; the
proportion who identified themselves as Jews was over thirteen times the
proportion of employed Americans; and three times as many answered 'No Religion'
as did all employed Americans. Volokh also observed that even these statistics
greatly understated the irreligiosity of faculty members since most of those who
classify themselves as Christians or Jews are only nominally so." Farron then
goes on to list the number of faculty that self-identify as Republicans versus
Democrats, again showing little diversity in terms of political affiliationthe
Democrats far outnumbers the Republicans. Universities therefore are far less
diverse than the average business or organizationwith a tight grip on
maintaining a socialist or liberal leaning in most subject areas that could be
influenced by the strong opinions of the facultyincluding censorship of truly
diverse opinions such as homosexuality, environmental issues, race, etc.
The most troubling aspect of quotas is in areas such as medicine, engineering
and other applied sciences. The only way remaining to make sure that
professional practitioners are qualified is through professional certification
testing. As Farron notes: "A Rand Corporation study found that the rate at which
students become board certified is closely correlated with their MCAT (medical)
score and their undergraduate GPA, but not with their rate of graduation from
medical school; and that among medical school graduates, 49 percent of
minorities (80 percent of whom were Black) eventually became board certified,
compared with 80 percent of Whites and Asians. In other words, if a White and a
minority student have the same undergraduate GPA and LSAT score, the minority
student tends to do worse in law school. This over-prediction for minority
students is large. For example, if a Black and White have the same undergraduate
GPA and LSAT score, the White's grade average at one of the top ten law schools
will probably be one-half of a standard deviation higher than the Black's, an
extremely large difference; and that is despite affirmative grading. This is a
manifestation of a universal tendencyfor academic criteria to over-predict the
performance of lower scoring groups. In other words, academic criteria are
biased in favor of lower scoring groups."
This does not speak to the observation that anyone can keep taking exams over
and over again, until they become proficient at exam-taking in one specialized
area and squeak by with a passing grade. Intelligence tests are often criticized
on this very point; a person can get better at the test with practice. Of
course, psychometricians are aware of this, similar to teaching to the test, and
factor out increased scores that are due to practice.
The other observation, that for a White and a Black having equivalent test
scores, that Blacks do not do as well in school and in practice, suggests that
not only do Blacks have lower average mental ability, they also difference in
personality types like conscientiousness or openness. One can only speculate,
but the fact that they do less well in practice than they do on tests suggests
even more problems with going to a Black doctor or being represented by a Black
lawyer. There are real problems with relying on certification or the passing of
a bar exam alone.
Farron notes: "The same is true of job performance. The panel appointed by the
American Academy of Sciences to study the predictive accuracy of the General
Aptitude Test Battery, found, based on a review of approximately 700 studies,
that of Blacks and Whites who pass the test with the same score, 13 percent of
the Whites but 38 percent of the Blacks do poorly on the job."
The question of such outcomes is often countered by assuming that Blacks are
still raised in lower class households. The data however does not support this.
Farron notes that, "A Black in his early twenties in 1995 was more than twice as
likely to have parents who attended college than a Black in 1963 was to
have parents who graduated from high
school. Nevertheless, Blacks'
academic performance did not improve at all in those thirty-two years. One more
point should be made. Racial differences in bar exam pass rates would be much
greater if passing it were more difficult. In recent years, boards of bar
examiners in many states have recommended raising the passing grade on their
state's bar exam because the current passing grade is too low to ensure even a
minimum competence to practice law."
Farron points out that test scores have been and are the best predictors of
educational success and job performancewith a correlation of about 0.52~0.55
between SAT scores and grade point averages, and he goes on to warn that if
anyone states less of a correlation, they have not adjusted for restriction of
range. Restriction of range is an interesting complication in the IQ wars. For
example, if only already quite intelligent test takers are used to determine
correlations, the correlation has to be adjusted mathematically for the
restricted range of those taking the test. Psychology has somewhat of a problem
with this phenomenon because so mental experiments use college students for
numerous psychological tests that are meant to be generalized to the whole
population. College students however are not a random sample of the general
public.
Businesses discovered that intelligence tests were the single most effective way
of predicting performance for hiring and promotion. Farron notes that
intelligence tests grew to 90% in business in 1963, just before the big push for
equal rights for Blacks and the Great Society Programs. Over the years,
intelligence testing, even though it is allowed for the military, has been both
banned and upheld as acceptable by the courts in a never-ending battle to
promote quotas and dismantle them. As of late, the battle has tilted towards the
efficacy of using intelligence tests, and it is only when the courts ignore the
scientific data does the ban on testing prevail. (The criteria for allowing
intelligence testing is so complicated that like everything with regards to
race, it comes down to the ideology of the members of any specific court.)
Farron then takes on the 800 pound guerilla in the room of the battle over
quotas: "In 1999, the New York Times marked Martin Luther King Day
(January 18) with a predictable editorial: '[For] the abyss between whites and
blacks in income and wealth, one does not have to look far to find the damage
done by intolerance and discrimination.' However, according to the 2000 Census,
the median household income of American Blacks was $33,500; of non-Hispanic
Whites, $52,000; and of Asian Americans, $64,000. Therefore, Asian Americans
must be doing more damage to Black Americans by intolerance and discrimination
than are White Americans. In fact, Asian Americans must be damaging White
Americans by intolerance and discrimination. Every statistic that proves that
Whites are discriminating against Blacks and Hispanics also proves that Asians
are discriminating against Whites."
He could have added even more weight to the above by including Jews, and noting
that when it comes to wealth and income, Whites are about in the middle between
Jews and Blacks, so Jews must be "damaging White Americans by intolerance and
discrimination."
By far the best quote by Farron is when he notes how Blacks are always saying
that they do not have the same level of greed as Whites, or that they have a
different way of knowing, or that their value system is different, or that
Blacks do poorly because it is culturally acceptable to act un-White. He notes:
"Again, cultural values must be assessed by their results. For a person who is
intent on suicide, cyanide is preferable to champagne. No other culture has
proved to be even remotely as successful as 'White-male culture' at providing
food; clothing; medicines; travel; homes with plumbing, heating, and electrical
appliances; and other products and services. If rejecting this culture is as
valid as accepting it, then the people who reject it cannot complain if
they do not have these things. And why would people whose culture is not
competitive, work-centered, and goal-oriented be bothered if few of their
members are corporate executives or professionals?"
There is hope for Whites in taking back their rightful place in education and
the job market. Farron states, "John Skrentny observed that in the middle 1960s,
'Advocacy of racial preference was one of those "third rails" of American
politics: Touch it and you die.' Hugh Graham pointed out that advocacy of racial
preference has remained 'the third rail of American politics' ever since. The
reason is obvious from the innumerable opinion surveys about affirmative action.
Peter Schuck observed that
every one has found that 'opposition to, and anger over, affirmative action is
pervasive among the white public and is just as strong among whites on the
political left as among those on the political right.' 'No researcher in this
field doubts .. . that the public's opinion remains decidedly and intensely
negative.' Schuck also pointed out, '[I]f
the public knew how large affirmative
action preferences . . . actually are, opposition would probably be even more
intense than it is.' The public's hostility would also be much more intense if
it fully realized that most beneficiaries of affirmative action are descendants
of people who have received preferential treatment from the time they or their
ancestors entered the United States.
"Schuck observed (172) that affirmative action: 'has been sustained by strong
support by ethnic organizations, national media, leading educational
institutions, large corporations, government bureaucracies, mainstream
foundations and other opinion leaders. . . . Large corporations' strong support
for affirmative action might seem counterintuitive . . . [but affirmative
action] tend[s] to advantage large companies by imposing onerous reporting,
staffing, and other compliance costs on smaller competitors who cannot bear them
easily.'"
I will take issue with Farron's accounting of "large corporations' strong
support for affirmative action." I think, but cannot prove, that it has to do
more with the elite's affinity for being seen as above the masses, and buying
into the latest zeitgeist. They are simply above being impacted by affirmative
action themselves, so they prefer to lean towards those they run withwhich
includes not only other business elite but politicians, the media, Hollywood,
etc. Being for affirmative action is convenient and makes one appear to be a
better person in the eyes of others like youthose who are never affected by
quotas and disenfranchisement because they are the White and powerful. In
effect, they can practice White separatism and seldom have to deal with
minorities directly unless they are equally famous, wealthy or gifted.
Finally, the question of "closing the gap" between Blacks and Whites that was
celebrated in the 80's has vanished. Intervention programs help when children
are young, but they are not sustained as genetic control kicks in to form the
adult brainsuch as increasing the myelination fatty tissue around conducing
nerves. Whites have more myelination than Blacks, making signal processing
faster.
Then there is "teaching to the test," where Blacks are much more heavily coached
to do well in areas that are tested, while Whites are pursuing a broader
education. In addition, tests themselves are being changed, including tests for
firemen, police, etc. If the gap between White scores and Black scores are too
far apart, all one has to do is make the test a lot easier, where many Whites
will get perfect scores. As Farron explains it: "The reader will now understand
what the recentering of 1995 accomplished. By truncating the high end of
the [SAT] Verbal scale, where racial differences are greatest, it lessened the
average White/Black difference and greatly reduced it at the top. The racial gap
in average scores continued to increase, but at a slower rate than it would have
if the scores had not been recentered; and the number of Blacks with Verbal
scores of 750 and above rose from nine in 1993 to 250 in 1997.
Thus, the greatest reduction in racial differences was at elite universities,
where the debate over academic affirmative action is most intense. (Despite the
constant increase in the number of Blacks taking the SAT, the number of Blacks
with Verbal scores of 750 and above declined to 218 by 2002.)"
In summation then, when all of the factors are included, especially differences
in mental ability when people grow into adulthood where one's educational
attainment is put into use, Blacks remain about 15 IQ points below Whites, and
Jews remain about 10 to 15 IQ points above Whites. In sub-Saharan Africa, the
average IQ of Blacks is estimated to be about 70. These are enormous differences
in average intelligence because they mean that at the high end of the bell
curve, the greater the average of any group in intelligence, there will be far
more people in the very high IQ range.
In summation, Farron notes: "The reader will now appreciate that the studies I
mentioned in Chapter 15 of the predictive accuracy of the SAT for future income
and occupational success are merely drops in an ocean of studies that have
unanimously found that tests of intelligence are extremely accurate indicators
of future success. I have outlined mainly the work of Murray and Herrnstein
because they provide the most thorough and accessible sources of information on
this subject. Other accessible sources for the superior predictive accuracy of
intelligence tests over criteria for occupational success and job performance
are Seligman, Eysenck, and Jensen. Brand provides a bibliography for the
predictive accuracy of intelligence tests for emotional maturity and social
skills.
"The absence of any counter-evidence is evident from the fact that the two most
influential attacks on intelligence testsStephen Jay Gould's
The Mismeasure of Man
and Daniel Coleman's
Emotional Intelligence: Why it Can Matter More
than IQ had to resort to blatant lies
and misrepresentations. In fact, not one of the sources that Coleman cited to
denigrate intelligence tests says what he claimed it says. Most say the
opposite."
Farron's book is comprehensive and well researched. Leaving out numerous
anecdotal stories, he has compiled the data on intelligence testing, its
correlation with educational and occupational success, and how academia has
distorted and manipulated the data and the rational to maintain quotas based on
race. I would add that he failed to mention Linda Goffredson's extensive work on
the correlation between intelligence and many of life's beneficial outcomes like
good health, fewer car accidents, etc. Much of this material is available at her
website.
In addition, Robert Sternberg seems to have replaced the late Stephen Jay Gould
in trying to deny the importance of intelligence through obfuscation and
injecting such notions as the value of wisdom (whatever that is), along with
Howard Gardner's multiple intelligence hypotheses. Sternberg and Gardner et al.
remain as the only two propagandists trying to derail the importance of mental
ability. Of course, the vast majority of government, academia and the media
simply ignore the connection between genes and intelligence. They offer no
alternative theories because they simply ignore intelligence as a factor in
one's ability to learn.
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone