Teach Medical Students How To Be Placebos

Enjoy this guest post from Karan Chhabra, a medical student at Rutgers Robert Wood Johnson Medical School and co-founder of the blog Project Millennial. Connect with him on Twitter at @krchhabra. Placebos work. This isn't news. The term "placebo" was coined 60 years ago to describe how one-third of people respond to pills without any active drug in them. Twenty-five years later, we learned how they work: through endorphins produced by the body that work just like morphine. Today placebos are everywhere: from mothers kissing boo-boos to international drug trials. A recent paper, though, shows that all placebos aren't created equal. As expected, the authors found placebo pills effective for reducing migraines in about one-fifth of patients. But it gets better. Take sham acupuncture, which doesn't target traditional pressure points and doesn't penetrate the skin. Despite being "fake," sham acupuncture reduced migraines in 38% of patients, making it as effective as real migraine drugs. The authors also studied sham surgery, in which doctors give anesthesia and cut the skin, but stitch it back together without doing anything to the tissues underneath. These fake operations helped 58% of migraine patients, potentially even more than active drugs. What does this tell us? It depends on whom you ask. Some might say we need to figure out how to predict a good response to placebo (and are trying to do just that). Others might say we need to test more procedures against shams, to make sure they're effective. Yet others might say sham surgery is unethical outright. These are questions without quick answers. For the rest of us, what can the placebo effect teach us about medicine as a whole? No treatments - drugs, placebos, shams - exist in a vacuum. They're part of a complex ritual of storytelling and listening, examining and touching, teaching and prescribing. Patients expect this ritual and I, like any medical student, am working to master it. Yesterday's medicine might've been a prayer or a poultice; todays it's delivered on blue slips and under blue drapes. But one thing that hasn't changed is that ailing humans want intervention. And the benefits of medicine transcend pills and procedures. Words, touch, and hope can be therapeutic. Wielded inappropriately, they can do harm too. Medicine embraces lots of treatments without proven benefit. Many doctors still stent narrowed blood vessels in the heart when patients have stable chest pain, even though the right pills extend life just as much. (Stents do appear better at preventing pain - but is that another placebo effect?) Some patients with early prostate cancer are also receiving expensive proton beam therapy instead of conventional radiation, again without evidence of benefit. Some doctors may be recommending these treatments for the wrong reasons. But I imagine many are working to satisfy a basic human impulse: to act aggressively in the face of disease. Our bodies respond better to high-touch, high-tech interventions: we get more pain relief from a $2.50 placebo than one that costs a dime. But bigger is not always better. Bigger is often more likely to do harm, through costs or complications.

What I take away from placebo research is that how we do our job is just as important as what we do. The notion that drugs and surgery are the only treatment we can offer has become a self-fulfilling prophecy. Medical training and research are decidedly focused on what drugs to give when - knowledge necessary, but not sufficient, to serve our patients. This may distract us from the psychological and social mechanisms beneath the human response to treatment. Rather than inventing a new procedure that might not be more effective than sham, we should be inventing ways to get the benefits of a sham without cutting the skin.

Many of our most common yet most frustrating afflictions have a psychological component: for example back pain, irritable bowel syndrome, and of course headaches. So it makes sense that these conditions have been shown again and again to respond to the placebo effect. It's now our duty to figure out how we can put that power to good use. What combination of advice, empathy, and touch unlocks our body's natural painkillers? Can we be high-touch without being high-tech? Can that be taught? Experienced clinicians may have the answer without even knowing it. But students like myself could use a bit more training on how to be our patients' placebos.

Here is the original post:

Teach Medical Students How To Be Placebos

Related Posts

Comments are closed.