Open Letters
THE ORION PARTY
The Prometheus League
- Humanity Needs A World Government PDF
- Cosmos Theology Essay PDF
- Cosmos Theology Booklet PDF
- Europe Destiny Essays PDF
- Historical Parallels PDF
- Christianity Examined PDF
News Blogs
Euvolution
- Home Page
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
By MICHAEL PETERS
University of Guelph
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology (Vol. 49, No. 4)
Abstract
Rushton and Ankney (1995) show that new Magnetic Resonance
Imaging mri studies allow the statement that brain size and IQ
are correlated. However, the evidence does not justify statements
about the relation of brain size and IQ across race and sex.
Rushton & Ankney's comparisons of brain size across races are
based on brain weights corrected for body weights, but the
corrections are not justifiable for within-species comparisons.
Large variations of brain size within race groupings (as defined
by Rushton & Ankney), and large secular changes in brain size add
to doubts about the validity of Rushton & Ankney's arguments
about race/brain size/IQ relations. Rushton & Ankney suggest that
sex differences in brain size relate to sex differences in IQ and
spatial abilities; however, the sex differences in IQ do not
stand in proportion to differences in brain size, and spatial
performance in women does not correlate with brain size. Scaling
brain size across sexes remains an unsolved problem.
Rushton and Ankney (1995) suggest that I was in error when
rejecting the claim (Lynn, 1993) that a relation between brain size
and intelligence is firmly established. Since my note went to press
(Peters, 1993), several papers have appeared which indicate a sizeable
relation between brain size, as determined by mri scan, and IQ
(Andreasen, Flaum, Swayze, O'Leary, Alliger, Cohen, Ehrhardtm, &
Yuhet, 1993; Egan, Chiswick, Santosh, Naidu, Rimmington, & Bestet,
1994; Raz, Torres, Spencer, Millman, Baertschi, & Sarpel, 1993;
Wickett, Vernon, & Lee, 1994). Each of the above studies has some
problems posed by method and interpretations or by the findings
themselves. For example, Egan et al. found the highest correlation not
between brain matter and IQ, but between cerebrospinal fluid volume
and IQ (r = .8), an inexplicable result for those who argue that it is
brain matter which is correlated with IQ. Nevertheless, as a group,
these studies justify the conclusion that there is a positive and
sizable correlation between brain size and IQ. This lends credibility
to earlier claims by Willerman, Schultz, Rutledge, & Bigler, 1991;
1992) and supports Rushton & Ankney's critIQue. The new mri data are
important because studies that relate IQ to brain size as estimated
through cranial parameters remain contradictory. For example, Reed and
Jensen (1993) report an equivalent cranial capacity of 1550 cm3 for a
high IQ group (124-136) and 1549 cm3 for a low IQ group (87-111),
whereas studies listed by Wickett et al. (1994) report positive
correlations between cranial capacity and IQ.
Accepting the fact that MRI data document a significant correlation
between brain size and intelligence within demographically homogeneous
groups, the question of causality arises. Neural systems, by
definition, have evolved to interact with the environment, and the
very significant expansion of brain size after birth, driven by a
growth of synapses and cortical interconnections, is interactive with
environmental input (Bedi, Massey, & Smart, 1989; Jacobsen, 1991, pp.
266-270; Walsh, 1981). Thus, nutritional and environmental conditions
which foster good development of intelligence can be expected to
foster good physical brain development as well. My seemingly
nonsensical position, that under some conditions it is useful to
control for body size when looking at brain/IQ relations, is based on
the possibility that the relation between brain size and IQ is
confounded by nutritional and environmental conditions (cf.
Passingham, 1979; Rodriguez, Donnadien, Martinez, & Chavez, 1979).
This does not change the basic observations, but it does change
arguments about causation.
Sex, IQ and brain size
Rushton & Ankney reiterate the conclusion, already reached by Gould
(1981, p. 106), that women have absolutely much smaller, and
relatively somewhat smaller brains than men. I continue to disagree
with Rushton & Ankney on the issue of scaling. The statement that
allometric technIQues standard in comparative biology have been used
does not assure that these are appropriately used for between sex
comparisons. In the available brain size/IQ studies, the differences
in IQ are small or non-existent (Passingham, 1979; Raz et al., 1993;
Willerman et al., 1991), in spite of very large differences in brain
size. Three interpretations offer themselves: (a) women do have lower
IQ's than men after all (Lynn, 1994), (b) the gross brain size in
women is not a meaningful index for comparison because of difficulties
in scaling body/brain parameters across sexes and possible differences
in fine structure (Peters, 1991; Willerman et al., 1991), and (c)
women and men differ especially on specific spatial tasks (Rushton &
Ankney, 1995). Because most of the mri evidence is cast in terms of
standard IQ test/brain size relations, it is interesting to note that
the sex differences in Wechsler IQ which are summarized by Lynn are
very small relative to the sex differences in brain size. This
supports interpretation (b). Rushton & Ankney's point (c) is difficult
to evaluate at this point. Rushton & Ankney emphasize spatial ability,
and point out that there are significant sex differences in 3-d
spatial abilities, especially mental rotation performance. However,
Wickett et al. find no significant correlation between brain size and
mental rotation performance in their sample of women, and considerably
lower correlations between Wechsler performance IQ and brain size than
for Wechsler verbal IQ and brain size. Caution is advised in
attributing spatial performance to brain size or neuron counts because
such performance is very sensitive to practice; women can improve
their mental rotation performance by 30% to 50 % (Peters, Chisholm, &
Laeng, 1995) after only a single exposure to the test. Such rapid
improvement is difficult to reconcile with the idea that brain size,
within normal limits, is the limiting factor in 3-d spatial
performance.
Rushton and Ankney (1995) classify races into three groups, and this
has been a source of contention (e.g., Weizmann et al., 1990). The
issue of race classification is a major problem in anthropology
(Harrison, Tanner, Pilbeam, & Baker, 1988; p. 326) and cannot be
addressed here. Rushton & Ankney have adopted an essentially
operational definition that shares the strengths and weaknesses of
such definitions. To simplify things, I shall adopt the terms
Caucasoid, Mongoloid, and Negroid when referring to the data in
Rushton's work.
Much of Rushton and Ankney's (1995) case on racial differences in
brain size is based on estimates derived from cranial measures. Such
estimates are not without problems (Hoadley & Pearson, 1929; Wickett
et al., 1994, Willerman et al., 1991), and may have different validity
for men and women. As an illustration, Willerman et al. (1992), found
a significant correlation between brain size and head perimeter for
women, but not for men. Nevertheless, let us assume that cranial
measures do provide an imperfect but acceptable estimate of brain
size. Rushton & Ankney encounter several problems in the attempt to
generalize the mri/brain size/IQ data to the general context of racial
comparisons. One of the problems lies in the simplified grouping of
races, because this tends to give insufficient weight to within- group
variations.
Rushton's (1991) own data illustrate the point. Within the Caucasoid
grouping, a 1969 sample of Iranian soldiers is given an average
estimated cranial capacity of 1356 cm3, whereas a 1966 sample of
American Army soldiers has an average of 1470 cm3. Similarly, a 1967
us Air Force sample has 1539 cm3, whereas a 1975 German Air Force
sample has 1455 cm3. Within the Mongoloid grouping, a 1963 sample of
Thai soldiers has an estimated average of 1340 cm3, whereas a 1965
sample of South Vietnamese has 1299 cm3. All of these differences are
much larger than the differences obtained between Negroid (1449 cm3),
Caucasoid (1468 cm3), and Mongol- oid (1464 cm3) enlisted men from a
1988 us Army cohort using Rushton's (1992) terms and data. There are
also secular changes within a culture and racial group (Haug, 1984;
Miller & Corsellis, 1977). For example, us Air Force personnel
measured in 1967 had estimated brain sizes that exceeded by 68 cm3
values from a us Air Force sample drawn in 1950 (Rushton, 1991). In
face of such variation within groupings, between cultures for similar
groupings, and between different cohorts drawn within a culture,
generally valid statements about race differences are difficult to
make.
Two additional points need to be made. Rushton and Ankney (1995)
suggest that cranial capacity estimates for Mongoloid-, Caucasoid-,
and Negroid-Americans are 1416, 1380, and 1359 cm3, respectively,
indicating larger differences than Rushton's (1992) values given above
for these groups (1464, 1468, 1449cm3). The former values for the
three groups represent cranial capacity estimates which are based on
values corrected for body parameters (Rushton, 1992). To perform this
correction, Rushton used slopes for the log/log plot of brain against
body weight which are not appropriate for within- species comparisons
(Harvey, 1988). For comparison of individuals drawn from the same
species, a slope which is almost horizontal is appropriate, and should
be close to the .08 determined empirically by Reed and Jensen (1993).
This is borne out by other available evidence. Wickett et al. (1994)
state that for their sample of white women, it would appear that the
size of the brain is largely independent of body size (p. 836).
Similarly, Jerison (1979) found no significant association between
body weight or height and brain weight for men within the age range of
29 to 41 years of age. A conservative conclusion is that there is no
legitimate reason for using steep slopes in comparing brain/body size
relations across races. As a result, statements about brain size
differences between races should not rely on adjusted values, and it
is not appropriate to conclude that higher IQ's in Asians are linked
to larger brain size.
The issue of race/brain size/IQ invites a return to the sex/brain
size/IQ issue. Rushton's (1992) data show that the estimated cranial
capacity of Negroid-American men is some 13-14% higher than that of
Caucasoid-American women, even though the average IQ for the former is
presumably lower. How can this be integrated into a model of larger
brain => higher IQ without qualifying the meaning of brain size
comparisons across sexes, or revisiting the issue of what factors
other than brain size have a bearing on IQ? This question once again
emphasizes the unresolved issues of how brain weight/ body parameters
can be compared across sexes, races, and age cohorts.
Finally, the small absolute differences in brain size between
Mongoloids, Caucasoids, and Negroids in Rushton's (1992) data base
should be evaluated relative to cohort data. We have seen that values
from two Air Force samples drawn 17 years apart showed estimated brain
size differences that are larger than the differences reported between
races in the 1988 common age cohort. It is legitimate to ask whether
the contemporary Negroid- and Caucasoid-American samples described in
the common 1988 age cohort could not differ as much from each other in
undefined demographic and nutritional variables as the cohort samples
from 1950 and 1967 differed from each other.
I am not going to address the issue of racial differences in IQ
relative to brain size, because this cannot be resolved here. Clearly,
Rushton & Ankney feel that sufficient evidence is available to make
their point on racial differences in IQ. The issue is not whether such
differences can be observed; they are observed and they are marked and
important for a number of reasons. What to make of them is another
matter. If cohort differences across time and culture complicate
interpretation of brain size differences across races, the additional
uncontrolled effects of community variables in the determination of IQ
(Church & Katigbak, 1991; Coon, Carey & Fulker, 1992; Wachs, Moussa,
Bishry, Yunis, Sobhy, McCabe, Jerome, Galal, Harrison, & Kirksley,
1993) across races render conclusive statements about racial IQ
differences even more difficult, if not impossible at present.
Send correspondence to:
M. Peters, Ph.D
Deptartment of Psychology,
University of Guelph,
Guelph, Ontario
N1G 2W1
CANADA
E-mail: mpeters@uoguelph.ca.
----------------------------------------------------------------------
References
Andreasen, N.C., Flaum, M., Swayze, V., O'Leary, D.S., Alliger, R.,
Cohen, G., Ehrhardt, J., Yuh, W.T.C. (1993). Intelligence and brain
structure in normal individuals. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150,
130-134.
Bedi, K.S., Massey, R.F., & Smart, J.L. (1989). Neuronal and synaptic
measure- ments in the visual cortex of adults after undernutrition
during normal or artificial rearing. Journal of Comparative Neurology,
289, 89-98.
Church, A.T., & Katigbak, M.S. (1991). Home environment, nutritional
status, and maternal intelligence as determinants of intellectual
development in rural Philippine preschool children. Intelligence, 15,
49-78.
Coon, H., Carey, G., & Fulker, D.W. (1992). Community influences on
cognitive ability. Intelligence, 16, 169-188.
Egan, V., Chiswick, A., Santosh, C., Naidu, K., Rimmington, J.E., &
Best, J.J.K. (1994). Size isn't everything: a study of brain volume,
intelligence and auditory potentials. Personality and Individual
Differences, 17, 357-367.
Gould, S.J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: W.W. Norton.
Harrison, G.A., Tanner, J.M., Pilbeam, D.R., & Baker, P.T. (1988).
Human Biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Harvey, P.H. (1988). Allometric analysis and brain size. In H.J.
Jerison & I. Jerison (Eds.), Intelligence and evolutionary biology
(pp. 199-210). Berlin: Springer Verlag.
Haug, H. (1984). Der Einfluss der s„kularen Akzeleration auf das
Hirngewicht des Menschen und dessen Žnderung w„hrend der Alterung.
Gegenbaurs morphologisches Jahrbuch, 130, 481-500.
Hoadley, M.F., & Pearson, K. (1929). On measurement of the internal
diameter of the skull in relation, I to the prediction of its capacity
and, II to the `pre-eminence' of the left hemisphere. Biometrika, 21,
85-123.
Jacobsen, M. (1991). Developmental Neurobiology. New York: Plenum
Press.
Jerison, H.J. (1979). The evolution of diversity in brain size. In
M.E. Hahn, C. Jensen, & B.C. Dudek (Eds.) Development and evolution in
brain size (pp. 29-57). New York: Academic Press.
Lynn, R. (1994). Sex differences in intelligence and brain size: a
paradox resolved. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 257-271.
Lynn, R. (1993). Brain size and intelligence in man: a correction to
Peters. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 748-750.
Miller, A.K.H., & Corsellis, J.A.N. (1977). Evidence for a secular
increase in human brain weight during the past century. Annals of
Human Biology, 4, 253-257.
Passingham, R.E. (1979) Brain size and intelligence in man. Brain,
Behavior and Evolution, 16, 253-270.
Peters, M. (1991). Sex Differences in Human Brain Size and the General
Meaning of Differences in brain size. Canadian Journal of Psychology,
45, 507-522.
Peters, M. (1993). Still no convincing evidence of a relation between
brain size and intelligence in humans. Canadian Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 47, 751-756.
Peters, M., Chisholm, P., & Laeng, B. (1995). Spatial ability, student
gender and academic performance. Journal of Engineering Education, 84,
69-73.
Raz, N., Torres, I.J., Spencer, W.D., Millman, D., Baertschi, J.C., &
Sarpel, G. (1993). Neuroanatomical correlates of age-sensitive and
age-invariant cognitive abilities: an in vivo mri investigation.
Intelligence, 17, 407-422.
Reed, T.E., & Jensen, A.R. (1993). Cranial capacity: new Caucasian
data and comments on Rushton's claimed Mongoloid-Caucasoid brain-size
differences. Intelligence, 17, 423-431.
Rodriguez, R., Donnadien, F.R., Martinez, C., & Chavez, A. (1979).
Nutrition and development of children from poor rural areas. viii. The
effect of mild malnutri- tion on children's neurological development.
Nutrition Reports International, 19, 315-326.
Rushton, J.P. (1991). Mongoloid-caucasoid differences in brain size
from military samples. Intelligence, 15, 351-359.
Rushton, J.P. (1992). Cranial capacity related to sex, rank and race
in a stratified random sample of 6325 u.s. military personnel.
Intelligence, 16, 401-413.
Wachs, T.D., Moussa, W., Bishry, Z., Yunis, F., Sobhy, A., McCabe, G.,
Jerome, N., Galal, O., Harrison, G., & Kirksley, A. (1993). Relations
between nutrition and cognitive performance in Egyptian toddlers.
Intelligence, 17, 151-172.
Walsh, R.N. (1981). Effects of environmental complexity and
deprivation on brain anatomy and histology. International Journal of
Neuroscience, 12, 33-51.
Weizmann, F., Wiener, N.I., & Wiesenthal, D.L., & Ziegler, M. (1990).
Differential K theory and racial hierarchies. Canadian Psychology, 31,
1-13.
Wickett, J.C., Vernon, P.A., & Lee, D.H. (1994). In vivo brain size,
head per- imeter, and intelligence in a sample of healthy adult
females. Personality and Individual Differencess, 16, 831-838.
Willerman, L., Schultz, R., Rutledge, & Bigler, E.D. (1991). In vivo
brain size and intelligence. Intelligence, 15, 223-228.
Willerman, L., Schultz, R., Rutledge, N.J., & Bigler, E.D. (1992).
Hemisphere size asymmetry predicts relative verbal and nonverbal
intelligence differently in the sexes: an mri study of
structure-function relations. Intelligence, 16, 315-328.
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone