Open Letters
THE ORION PARTY
The Prometheus League
- Humanity Needs A World Government PDF
- Cosmos Theology Essay PDF
- Cosmos Theology Booklet PDF
- Europe Destiny Essays PDF
- Historical Parallels PDF
- Christianity Examined PDF
News Blogs
Euvolution
- Home Page
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
A Substantial Inheritance
By Daniel Seligman
from National Review, October 10, 1994
As a result of genetic research, human nature is making a
comeback.
Hereditarianism is on the march. Nature is clobbering nurture. A steady
drip, drip, drip of scientific studies is cumulatively telling us that more
and more human traits are genetically influenced. Some of the findings are
based on studies of twins and adoptions; others have been generated by
research in molecular biology and related hard sciences. The media have shown
a particular interest in recent data linking genes to sexual orientation,
alcoholism, violent and criminal behavior, and obesity, not to mention
cheating on wives. "Infidelity: It may be in our genes," proclaimed the August
15 Time cover. The cover story, by Robert Wright, was based on his new book,
The Moral Animal: Evolutionary Psychology and Everyday Life, a work heavily
influenced by the science of sociobiology — which has also generated a lot of
data linking genes to human behavior.
Some of the nature - nurture news stories also touch on IQ, although you
would have difficulty deducing from the coverage that in this area there has
been no serious dispute for decades about a powerful genetic effect. The
August 9 Boston Globe — which was bracing its readers for The Bell Curve:
Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life, by Richard J. Herrnstein
and Charles Murray (to be published in October) — had a headline that could
have appeared forty years ago: "IQ Fight Renewed; New Book Links Genes,
Intelligence."
Curiously unnoticed by the reporters and anchorpersons of America is my own
favorite finding of recent years: that political beliefs are strongly
influenced by genes. The finding, exhaustively documented in the twin study
program at the University of Minnesota, asks you to imagine a continuum of
political attitudes. At one end are instinctive conservatives, here conceived
as people who tend to respect traditional values and established authority; at
the other end are rebellious types generally inclined to kick over the traces.
One's place on this continuum is established by responses to a battery of
questions gauging attitudes toward conservatism. It turns out that the test
scores of identical twins (who are, of course, genetically indistinguishable)
correlate far more closely than do the scores of fraternal twins (who have
only about half their genes in common), even when the identical twins were
reared apart and the fraternal twins were brought up together in the same
household.
The media's rendering of the news about genes has been uneven, incomplete
(especially in dealing with male - female differences), and maddeningly
misleading in major respects. Still, there is no doubt that the literate
public has been assimilating a few large truths: that genes play a greater
role in human behavior than previously posited; that human beings are somewhat
less malleable than had been assumed; that human nature is making something of
a comeback.
Onward to Utopia
THE centrality of human nature, a.k.a "instinct," was received wisdom in
psychology and anthropology early in this century. It was very much onstage in
the world's first serious psychology textbook, William James's Principles of
Psychology (1890), a work that drew heavily on Darwinian parallels between
human and animal behavior. The Darwinian paradigm remained dominant for many
decades.
By mid century, however, this model was pretty much undone in the realm of
ideas. It was fighting Marxism and Freudianism, whose alternative visions both
featured human behavior shaped by the environment. In addition, the
master-race version peddled by the Nazis had made hereditarianism much harder
to defend. It was gradually supplanted by a commitment to one or another form
of cultural determinism. In Search of Human Nature, by Carl N. Degler of
Stanford, traces the rise of this new model to anthropologist Franz Boas, who
had been assailing hereditarian ideas as early as 1910 and whose students and
disciples increasingly nudged the thinking classes toward a model of human
development in which "culture," rather than biology, was supreme. By the
1950s, anthropologist Ashley Montagu was proclaiming that man "has no
instincts, because everything he is and has become he has learned, acquired,
from his culture." In 1961 the president of the American Sociological Society
hailed "the new optimism," identified as a conviction that "anybody can learn
anything."
This expansive view of human malleability was exactly what numerous social
engineers were eager to hear in the Sixties, and it still lingers in
high-minded rhetoric about educational reform. In 1987, when he was the chief
executive of Xerox, David Kearns made a speech calling for " a new national
agenda" and proposing, incredibly, that "every student — without exception —
should master a core curriculum equivalent to college entrance requirements."
Possibly owing to his utopian credentials, Kearns later became deputy
secretary of education in the Bush Administration.
Adapting to the era of limited malleability has not been easy for the
media. First, there has been endless confusion about and misrepresentation of
the data. One keeps reading that the evidence points to homosexuality being
"immutable, not a personal choice" (Los Angeles Times), or that "sexual
or-ientation is innate" (New York Times), or that it is "biologically
determined" (Boston Globe). Or, when the subject is data pointing to genetic
and biochemical markers for violent behavior, that "biology is destiny"
(Time). Or, in news stories about a hereditary basis for obesity, that a
particular gene "is the cause of" compulsive eating (St. Louis Post-Dispatch).
The principal difficulty with all these formulations — in some cases, they
are hedged or qualified elsewhere in the article I am quoting — is that none
of the data now emerging postulates any such determined outcomes. The news is
about probabilities, not about "destiny." In every case the data concern
genetic effects that "predispose" one in this or that direction and thereby
change the odds of particular outcomes. They represent new estimates of the
"heritabilities" involved in the trait. The heritability of obesity, for
example, is apparently somewhere around 0.40, meaning that 40 per cent of the
population's variability in body weight is attributable to genes, leaving 60
per cent for environmental effects. (Obesity is generally defined as 20 per
cent or more overweight in relation to height and body type. ) For
homosexuality the heritability may be as high as 0.50. Some scholars say it is
in about the same zone for alcoholism. (Others are profoundly skeptical of any
genetic influence at all in alcoholism.) For political attitudes it is about
0.60, a figure raising the question of whether ideological sperm banks are
just over the horizon. For IQ the heritability is even higher, by some
measures as high as 0.80.
A second, related problem with the press coverage is its insistent
politicization of the data. Over and over again, one sees the media spin
doctors gravitating to questions about the political implications of the news:
whether it is good or bad for this or that politically correct cause, and, if
bad, whether such research should be continued.
This was particularly the case with data suggesting a biological basis for
violent crime. The existence of such data has been documented in many
different ways. Studies have repeatedly shown identical twins to be more alike
than fraternal twins in various measures of criminality. It is clear that
several traits associated with violent criminals — muscular physique, low IQ,
and impulsiveness — are strongly influenced by genes. Dr. Markku Linnoila of
the National Institutes of Health has spent many years building a data base
relating deficiencies in serotonin (a brain-based chemical that facilitates
transmissions between neurons) to impulsive violent behavior, and almost
nobody doubts he is on to something.
The Nazi Tradition?
THE BIG issue about such studies nowadays is not so much their validity as
the permissibility of pursuing them at all. The hangup here is racial:
Justice Department data indicate that blacks, who represent about 12 per
cent of the U.S. population, commit about half of all violent crimes (defined
as murder, non-negligent manslaughter, rape, aggravated assault, and robbery).
Which raises the prospect that any research into the genetic and/or biological
roots of violent crime would at some point be addressing differences in racial
propensities. Numerous scholars are determined that no such research be done,
and scholars wishing to do it are endlessly told that they are acting in the
Nazi tradition.
Prominent among those making such points is Dr. Peter Breggin, founder of
the Center for the Study of Psychiatry, who was recently quoted in the Atlanta
Journal and Constitution as concerned that the research would turn into a
witch hunt against inner-city black kids. He added: "For America to suggest
that the problem lies in them is hypocritical and evil, and to think of doing
genetic studies in our inner cities is very close to the Nazi philosophy of
blaming and oppressing the victim." Two years ago, the NIH was supporting a
conference, to be held at the University of Maryland, on genetic factors in
crime. Breggin howled, as did the Congressional Black Caucus. NIH Director
Bernadine Healy instantly caved, and the conference was never held.
Political correctness has also been onstage in coverage of the data on
gays. In this instance, however, there have been no demands for suppression of
the data, which the gay-rights movement generally finds congenial. The new
findings here have mainly been identified with two researchers. One is
neurobiologist Simon LeVay, who in 1991, when he was at the Salk Institute in
La Jolla, reported that a particular cell cluster in the hypothalamus was
smaller in gay men than in straight men. The other is Dean Hamer of the
National Cancer Institute, who reported in Science last year that he had found
differences in the DNA of gay and straight men. Both LeVay and Hamer have
repeatedly stated that their research does not point to a "gay gene" and does
not imply that homosexuality is determined before birth.
Why, then, would so many media accounts create the opposite impression?
Doubtless a contributing factor is the difficulty so many newsrooms have in
dealing with complex quantitative data. But I believe that the main reason is
political: the concept of a predetermined sexual orientation offered
irresistible polemical opportunities to PC editorialists. For openers, it gave
them a chance to beat the "Radical Right" over the head. If evangelicals say
that homosexuality is "immoral," that must mean they believe gays have a
choice in their sexual orientation. So it would be nice to argue that no
choice is involved — gayness, no less than straightness, is a God-given trait.
As elaborated by a Boston Globe editorialist: "The arguments of homophobes
usually imply that homosexuals are somehow making a perverted choice. But the
findings of Hamer's team . . . would tend to show that homosexuality . . . is
biologically determined. . . . It could ease the struggle to secure equal
protection for all Americans, regard less of sexual orientation."
The notion of a biologically determined sexual orientation had another
attractive implication for progressive journalists. It meant that parents
could no longer rationally defend their objections to gay influences in their
children's lives. As Time argued in an article a year ago (July 26, 1993):
"Parents might be more relaxed about allowing children to have gay teachers,
Boy Scout leaders, and other role models, on the assumption that the child's
future is written in his or her genetic makeup." Note, however, that this case
crumbles fast as we move from biologically determined outcomes to mere
tendencies. If a boy had any predisposition to gayness, his parents would
possibly be more concerned about gay Scoutmasters than if they had never heard
of the new research.
An amusing footnote to these arguments emerged from some comments made by
Dean Hamer at last winter's San Francisco meeting of the American Association
for the Advancement of Science. At a news conference, Hamer expressed concern
about one possible application of his research. He raised the possibility that
the findings would lead eventually to prenatal tests for the predisposition to
homosexuality, worried that some parents might elect to abort any fetuses at
risk of being gay, and said he hoped to patent the gene in question and
prevent homophobic parents from misusing his research. His position was widely
reported, and applauded, and my search in Nexis turned up a non-amazing
non-event. There were no editorials saying Hamer's plan was in conflict with a
woman's right to abort unwanted pregnancies.
'Anything You Can Do . . .'
POLITICAL agendas are also discernible in the media treatment of data on
male - female differences. The press has done fairly well at rendering the
work of Harvard psychologist Carol Gilligan — Nexis was recently offering 547
articles that mention her — and especially the core concepts of her book In a
Different Voice, which portrays women as far more empathetic and "caring" than
men. This thought, which had arguably occurred to your grandmother long before
Professor Gilligan got around to it, has now been assimilated by most feminist
thinkers. But the media and modern feminism are still rigidly rejecting the
avalanche of data depicting basic differences in male and female intellectual
skills.
A striking instance of the rejection was the colossally uninformed coverage
of the lawsuit last winter in which the American Civil Liberties Union and the
National Center for Fair & Open Testing called upon the U.S. Department of
Education to declare the Scholastic Aptitude Test in violation of Title IX of
the Education Amendments of 1972, which bars sex discrimination in federally
funded education. The suit's basic proposition: that the SAT (the name has
been changed, so that the "A" now stands for Assessment) obviously
discriminates against young women. Principal evidence: that women represent 55
per cent of the high-school juniors taking the preliminary SAT but only 40 per
cent of those whose test scores qualify them for National Merit Scholarships.
To qualify, you have to be above the 98th percentile of the testees.
A thought that was almost impossible to find in media coverage of this
event was that this is precisely what serious students of male - female
differences would have expected. There is broad (not quite total) agreement
that men and women are on average equal in mental ability: they have different
strengths and weaknesses, with a huge advantage for men in spatial abilities,
which are deeply implicated in mathematical talent, and an offsetting verbal
advantage for women. Camilla Benbow of Iowa State University is among the
numerous scholars who believe these differences have a biological basis.
If the sexes are on average equal in ability, why would men be dominant
among the National Merit Scholarship winners? Because in virtually all mental
domains, males are more variable than females, i.e., the distribution of their
scores is less bunched around the mean. David Lubinski of Iowa State and
Professor Benbow, two prominent researchers who have studied the variability
issue, have analyzed the test scores of several hundred thousand high-school
students and concluded that even in domains where females have a higher
average, males will be more variable. Obvious implication: in any sizable
group of gifted (or retarded) students, you would expect males to be
overrepresented.
I said above that it was "almost impossible" to find this thought in the
media. In fact, I stumbled upon it in only one place: in a publictelevision
discussion program called To the Contrary. The program has only female
discussants, and on the day I tuned in one of them was Linda Chavez, who said
that the National Merit Scholarship results were not surprising, since the
greater male variability was well established. To be sure, Miss Chavez is a
conservative and an occasional NATIONAL REVIEW contributor.
Taking everything together, the emerging limits-to-malleability perspective
looks like better news to conservatives than to liberals. Down through the
years, conservatives have almost always been less attracted to political
initiatives — public housing, penal rehabilitation, the Job Corps, Head Start,
international Communism — that were in some measure advertised as creating new
and better kinds of human beings. Conservatives tend to be far gloomier than
leftists and liberals in judging the possibilities of changing mankind. In A
Conflict of Visions, published in 1987, Thomas Sowell argued persuasively that
their different perspectives on human nature were fundamental to their
disagreements on a wide range of public-policy questions. Contrasting the
utopianism of the Left with the "constrained vision" of the Right, Sowell
wrote: "What fundamentally distinguishes the two visions is their respective
perceptions of human potential."
In the IQ debate, or at least that portion of it centering on the nature -
nurture issue, conservatives have generally seemed quite comfortable with data
running up the score for nature, possibly because the evidence confirms their
intuitive doubts about so many ameliorative social programs. By the same
token, strenuous resistance to the data tends to come from scholars on the
Left. Typically they have been Marxists, Stephen Jay Gould of Harvard and Leon
Kamin of Northeastern being among the more prominent. The single most
hard-line statement against a genetic basis for IQ is still Not in Our Genes,
a 1984 work by R. C. Lewontin, Steven Rose, and Leon Kamin, who posit that IQ
studies are a weapon employed by the ruling class to hold down the poor and
minorities, and who seem unable to discuss the human condition without
dragging in Marx, Engels, Feuerbach, and "revolutionary philosophers and
practitioners like Mao Tse-tung." Kamin was one of the scholars turned to by
the Boston Globe for its recent report on the Herrnstein - Murray book. He was
quoted as stating that the book was "politics masquerading as science."
Guaranteed: no shortage of politics as the gene data unfold.
Mr. Seligman, a Fortune columnist, is the author of A Question of
Intelligence: The IQ Debate in America (Citadel)
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone