Open Letters
Yggdrasil's Library
THE ORION PARTY
The Prometheus League
- Humanity Needs A World Government PDF
- Cosmos Theology Essay PDF
- Cosmos Theology Booklet PDF
- Europe Destiny Essays PDF
- Historical Parallels PDF
- Christianity Examined PDF
News Blogs
Euvolution
- Home Page
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
Thomas J. Bouchard. Science, June 17, 1994 v264 n5166 p1700(2)
Brief Summary: Twin studies indicate that genetic differences explain about
two-thirds of the reliable variance in personality traits such as
extraversion, neuroticism and conscientiousness. The next step is to determine
the function of individual personality differences in evolution.
The idea that genetic factors influence behavior, including personality, is
very old. The most compelling evidence has always been, as Darwin[1] noted,
the successful domestication of animals.
So in regard to mental qualities, their transmission is manifest in our
dogs, horses and other domestic animals. Besides special tastes and habits,
general intelligence, courage, bad and good tempers, etc., are certainly
transmitted. Unlike genetic influences on the intelligence quotient, which
have been studied continuously since the time of Galton a century ago, the
study of genetic influences on personality has had a much briefer history.
Although Galton discussed genetic influence on personality, the lack of
reliable and valid measures of personality qualities hampered progress. In
addition, until recently, psychologists could not agree on which were the
important traits of personality. Currently there is a modest consensus that
five broad traits or "super factors" are necessary to describe
personality-extraversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and
openness (2) (see table).
Until the early 1980s, the evidence for genetic influence on personality
derived almost exclusively from twin studies that utilized very modest sample
sizes and measured different variables. Heritability was estimated as twice
the difference between the correlation for identical or monozygotic (MZ) twins
and that for fraternal or dizygotic (DZ) twins. The typical conclusion was
that about 50% of the observed variance in personality is due to genetic
factors [3]. The influence on personality of the shared home environment
(estimated as twice the DZ correlation minus the MZ value) was concluded to be
small or even negligible. These simple equations make a number of assumptions,
including (i) on average DZ twins share half as many genes in common by
descent as MZ twins, (ii) the genes act additively, and (iii) MZ and DZ twins
experience the same shared environmental influences. If the assumptions are
correct, the difference between the two types of twins reflects one-half the
genetic influence on the trait being studied.
The conclusion that 50% of the variation in personality is genetic was not
universally embraced. Many psychologists questioned that MZ and DZ twins
experience the same home environment and ascribed much of the greater
similarity of MZ twins over DZ twins to more similar environmental treatment
of the MZ than the DZ twins. It also seemed implausible to psychologists that
being reared in the same home would have so little influence on sibling
similarity. Consequently these findings were not generally accepted outside of
behavioral genetics.
In recent years, three trends have converged to transform our understanding
of genetic and environmental influences on personality traits. First, studies
of twins reared together with very large sample sizes, in some instances over
2000 pairs of each sex and zygosity, have been carried out. Second, data have
been gathered from monozygotic and dizygotic twins reared apart (MZA and DZA),
as well as from both biological and adoptive families. Third, powerful methods
of model fitting have been introduced that allow full utilization of the
available information and statistical testing of competing hypotheses (4, 5).
The figure compares the results of the early twin studies, an analysis of
an extremely large data set assembled by Loehlin (6), and our own analysis of
MZA (n = 59) and DZA (n = 47) data from the Minnesota study of twins reared
apart (MISTRA) and MZT (n = 522) and DZT (n = 408) twins from the Minnesota
Twin Registry (7). The Loehlin analysis yields an estimated genetic influence
of 42% (with a sizable contribution from nonadditive genetic factors -
influences that are configural and not inherited in a simple additive manner)
and a very modest contribution of the shared environment. The most
parsimonious fit to the Minnesota data is a simple additive genetic model for
all five traits with an estimate of genetic influence of 46%. Addition of
nonadditive genetic and shared environmental parameters do not, however,
significantly change the fit of the model, and those data are shown in the
figure for comparison with the Loehlin analysis. Both approaches yield
estimates of genetic influence of just over 40% and modest estimates of shared
environmental influence (7%). Of the remaining variance, about half is due to
nonshared environmental influences and half to error of measurement. Thus,
about two-thirds of the reliable variance in measured personality traits is
due to genetic influence.
The early studies of twins appear to have only slightly overestimated the
degree of genetic influence on personality variation, and the main
contribution of the more sophisticated recent analyses is that some of the
genetic influence seems to be due to nonadditive genetic variance for all five
traits. All three analyses yield quite small estimates of shared environmental
influence. This is now a well-replicated finding in behavior genetics, and its
implications are straightforward. The similarity we see in personality between
biological relatives is almost entirely genetic in origin. If we wish to study
environmental influences on personality development in families, we must look
for influences that operate differentially among children in the same family
(8).
However, simply demonstrating that systematic differences in treatment
within the family exist does not suffice to prove that such treatments explain
personality differences. First, the treatment may have no effect. For example,
differences in socialization due to birth order exist, but contrary to
widespread belief[9], they do not influence personality[10]. Second, as
Lytton(11) has demonstrated, the differential behavior of children is often
the cause of differential parental behavior rather than a consequence. Third,
arguments as to the purported importance of environmental factors in shaping
personality, though superficially plausible, often fail to stand up to
scrutiny when subjected to quantitative analysis. Consider physical
attractiveness. It is often argued that because twins reared apart are similar
in physical attractiveness they must be treated alike, and therefore this is
an important source of their similarity in personality[9, 12]. The problem
with this argument is that physical attractiveness is so poorly correlated
with personality traits that, when numbers are fit to the model implied by the
argument, it can explain only a trivial portion of the similarity between MZA
twins (8, 10). In truth, how nontraumatic environmental determinants influence
the normal range of variance in adult personality remains largely a mystery.
This variation may even turn out to be the equivalent of noise[13].
Current thinking holds that each individual picks and chooses from a range
of stimuli and events largely on the basis of his or her genotype and creates
a unique set of experiences
* that is, people help to create their own environments[14]. This view of
human development does not deny the existence of inadequate and debilitating
environments nor does it minimize the role of learning. Rather, it views
humans as dynamic creative organisms for whom the opportunity to learn and to
experience new environments amplifies the effects of the genotype on the
phenotype. It also reminds us of our links to the biological world and our
evolutionary history. This brings us to the core problem of the genetics of
personality - the function of the variation in personality traits. The purpose
of this variation is undoubtedly rooted in the fact that humans have adapted
to life in face-to-face groups (sociality). Unraveling the role human
individual differences play in evolution is the next big hurdle[15], and its
solution will turn the behavior genetics of human personality from a
descriptive discipline to an explanatory one.
References
[1] C. Darwin, The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (Murray,
London, 1871; reprinted by Modern Library, New York, 1967).
[2] L. R. Goldberg, Am. Psychol. 48, 26 (1993).
[3] R. C. Nichols, Homo 29, 158 (1978).
[4] L. J. Eaves, H. J. Eysenck, N. G. Martin, Genes, Culture and
Personality. An Empirical Approach (Academic Press, New York, 1989).
[5] M. C. Neale and L. R. Cardon, Eds., Methodology for Genetic Studies of
Twins and Families Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, 1992).
[6] J. C. Loehlin, Genes and Environment in Personality Development (Sage,
Newbury Park, CA, 1992).
[7] T. J. Bouchard Jr., D. T. Lykken, M. McGue, N. L. Segal,
1. Tellegen, Science250 , 223 (1990).
[8] D. Rowe, The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience, and
Behavior Guilford, New York, 1994).
[9] L. W. Hoffman, Psychol. Bull. 110 , 1 87 (1991).
[10] T. J. Bouchard Jr., in Basic Issues in Personality, I. Deary and J.
Hettema, Eds. (Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, 1993).
[11] H. Lytton, Dev. Psychol. 26 , 705 (1990).
[12] B. D. Ford, Am. Psychol. 48, 1294 (1993).
[13] P. C. M. Molenar, D. I. Boomsma, C. V. Dolan, Behav. Genet. 6, 519
(1993).
[14] S. Scarr, Child Dev. 63, 1 (1992).
[15] D. M. Buss, in Twins as a Tool of Behavioral Genetics, T. J. Bouchard
Jr. and P. Propping, Eds. (Wiley, Chichester, 1993).
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone