Open Letters
THE ORION PARTY
The Prometheus League
- Humanity Needs A World Government PDF
- Cosmos Theology Essay PDF
- Cosmos Theology Booklet PDF
- Europe Destiny Essays PDF
- Historical Parallels PDF
- Christianity Examined PDF
News Blogs
Euvolution
- Home Page
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
J. Philippe Rushton
Society, March-April 1994 v31 n3 p40(5)
Brief Summary: Burt was attacked by social reform advocates because his
research on twins found a correlation between genetics and intelligence. This
upset people who believed that environmental factors determined intelligence.
Recent studies support Burt's original conclusions.
Cyril Burt's report of a preponderant genetic contribution to mental
ability in monozygotic twins raised apart, flew in the face of two of this
century's most powerful ideas: environmentalism and genetic equalitarianism.
In 1961 Henry Garrett, a president of the American Psychological Association,
referred to these as the "equalitarian dogma." In its strongest form, this
dogma holds that all social groups classes, races, and sexes - are genetically
created equal in intellectual capacity and that disproportionate achievement
was entirely the result of opportunity and other social factors.
Cyril Burt is featured in many psychology textbooks, not for his
scientific discoveries, which were many, but for his alleged misrepresentation
of data. By implication, the genetic basis of giftedness and intelligence is
then held still not to be established. Meanwhile, new evidence from studies of
twins raised apart have corroborated Burt's high heritability estimate, as
have independent data from adoption and other family studies. Examination of
the relationship of brain size to intelligence, and of race, sex, and social
rank differences in brain size, suggest that Henry Garrett was correct to
label the equalitarian dogma "the scientific hoax of the century." Cyril Burt
was one of many victims of this hoax.
Denial of racial differences in IQ seems to have been what mostly fuelled
the attack on Burt's integrity. Burt was concerned with differences of social
class and only rarely strayed into discussion of race or gender differences.
He held that the British upper classes contained a larger proportion of high
genetic intelligence than did the British lower classes, even though, in
absolute numbers, there would be more gifted children outside of the upper
classes than inside of them. (Child prodigies of humble origin were of special
interest to him.) Because of his belief in the degree of overlap in the
distributions, and also because of his belief that entrance into advanced
school systems should be based on test and examination performance rather than
the privilege of birth, Burt was considered a liberal in his day.
Burt's "day" was the 1920s and 1930s. He was born in 1883, the son of a
medical doctor, and entered Oxford University in 1902 to read classics. While
there he became enamored with the psychology of mental ability, a passion that
was to last throughout his long life. He was a student of William McDougall,
the instinct theorist and helped to collect data for Francis Galton, cousin to
Charles Darwin. In 1913, Burt became an educational psychologist for London
County Council. In 1924 he became professor of educational psychology, and in
1932 he succeeded to the chairmanship of the psychology department at
University College. At this time he began to publish his studies showing a
high heritability for IQ. In 1946 he was knighted by the Labour Government for
his work on psychological testing and for making educational opportunities
more widely available.
Burt broke new ground with the study of environmental effects, researching
many family factors. In The Backward Child, published in 1937, he separated
environmental variables of deprivation, such as poor nutrition and illness,
from the innate factors that handicapped children. He advocated medical and
dental examinations within the school setting to ensure that growth was
proceeding normally and he was partly responsible for the daily distribution
of milk to ensure adequate levels of vitamin D and the elimination of the
scourge of rickets. In addition to malnutrition, he identified other physical
causes of poor concentration such as defects of hearing, sight, speech, spinal
curvature. He was one of the first to correlate sociological factors with poor
school achievement, finding high relationships with residential indicators of
infantile mortality, overcrowding, poverty, unemployment, family size, and the
host of variables now only too familiar.
Burt was also interested in the factors affecting children at the top end
of the scale. In The Gifted Child, published four years after his death, Burt
focused on the damaging effects to a bright child, and to society, if the
intelligence was not recognized because of the poverty of a child's
background, inefficiency of the school system, or temperamental traits such as
laziness in the child. Burt advocated special teaching and special classes,
even special schools, for the gifted. He disapproved of the bias against the
whole notion of giftedness manifested by equalitarianism.
Burt retired officially in 1950 but continued his scholarly activity. From
1947 to 1967 he was editor of the British Journal of Statistical Psychology.
He also continued to publish data on the heritability of mental ability,
including data from identical twins raised apart. These studies consistently
suggested a large genetic contribution. Burt died in 1971 at the age of
eighty-eight. His last book, The Gifted Child, was published posthumously in
1975.
The "Burt Affair" began in 1973 when Leon Kamin, then at Princeton
University, claimed to have found discrepancies in some of Burt's figures,
including an invariantly high correlation for IQ scores in twins raised apart.
Despite the increase in sample size, from fifteen pairs in 1943 to fifty-three
pairs in 1966, the correlation remained at a rounded 0.77. The scandal broke
wide open with a story in the Sunday Times in 1976 headlined "Crucial Data Was
Faked by Eminent Psychologist." The article charged not only that Burt had
adjusted his data to suit his theory but that two of Burt's collaborators "may
never have existed." The controversy flared for about three years. Then Burt's
biographer Leslie Hearnshaw, a respected historian of psychology with access
to Burt's private correspondence and diaries, concluded that Burt was
"guilty." In 1980, the British Psychological Society, refusing to conduct an
inquiry of its own, endorsed the guilty verdict. Even Burt's hereditarian
defenders, Hans Eysenck in London and Arthur Jensen at Berkeley, withdrew
their support.
The battle seemed over with an enormous victory for the equalitarians.
Then, suddenly, in 1989, Robert B. Joynson re-opened the case and concluded
that the accusations of fraud were ill-founded and that Burt must be
exonerated. Working independently, Ronald Fletcher completed the demolition of
the evidence for the prosecution, concluding with a "not proven." Fletcher
drew out the implications, describing how ideology, in alliance with a
receptive popular journalism and the media, established itself as a powerful
third force in scientific discourse.
Many of the details of the case are fascinating and disturbing. For
example, there is the truly "flabbergasting" fact (Jensen's term) that many of
Burt's papers were destroyed by his housekeeper almost immediately after his
death on the advice of Liam Hudson, professor of educational psychology at
Edinburgh University, one of Burt's most ardent opponents. As Jensen has
stated: "Both Hudson's rush to Burt's flat right after his death and his
advice to Burt's secretary-housekeeper to burn the stored data seem stranger
than fiction. Surely it must be one of the most bizarre events in the whole
Burt affair."
On the most important issues, the matter appears settled. As for the
so-called "missing" research assistants, they have been found. Of even greater
importance, there have now been six studies of monozygotic twins raised apart.
As Jensen, among others, has pointed out, Burt's data are by no means out of
line with other findings. If an average is taken of the five other studies,
weighted by sample size, the result is 0.75, almost the same as Burt's
supposedly faked correlation of 0.77. Findings such as these led Sandra Scarr
to title her 1986 presidential address to the Behavior Genetics Association
"Three Cheers for Behavioral Genetics." She observed that "the war [between
nature and nurture] is largely over." Scarr accepted that genetics underlay
existing white social-class differences in IQ in the United States and Western
Europe, although this may not have been the case for earlier generations when
social mobility was more restricted. Large surveys have shown that a majority
of experts believe that Scarr's opinion is correct and that the heritability
of IQ in the American white population is about 60 percent, as reported by
Mark Snyderman and Stanley Rothman.
The experts have been more cautious, however, in the matter of race. In
her 1986 address, Scarr rejected a genetic explanation because racial barriers
were less permeable than class barriers. She interpreted her own work as
having shown an environmental cause for racial variation. I do not know
whether she changed her opinion as a result of the recent debate, in the
journal Intelligence, over her follow-up of black seventeen-year-olds raised
by white, middle-class parents, in which the black, white, and mixed-race
children's IQ scores are more accurately predicted by their biological origin
than by the environment in which they were brought up.
It may seem strange that Burt should have been considered a liberal in his
day. It will be salutory to remind ourselves of just how different the world
of the 1970s (when Burt died) was from that of earlier decades.
Internationally, the political spectrum had shifted to the far left. Over
two-thirds of the world was ruled by communist or socialist dictatorships.
Socialism seemed to be the wave of the future. Social class was no longer the
issue.
With the demise of European imperialism and decolonization, the large
influx of non-white immigrants into Europe, the American Civil Rights
Movement, and the Vietnam War, race and gender became substitutes for social
class in the rhetoric of exploitation, oppression, and liberation. Opposition
to hierarchy generated the concept of "political correctness." Among the most
politically incorrect scientific possibilities are evolution-based, genetic
differences in brain size and intelligence between the races, the sexes, and
the social classes. Yet, most recent data sets have shown clear evidence for
exactly these group differences. Men's brains weigh an average of about 100
grams (8 percent) more than do women's brains, even after correction is made
for the differences in body size, and Asians and Asian-Americans average
proportionately larger brains than do Europeans and European-Americans who
average larger than do Africans and African-Americans. Although group
differences were widely believed to exist in the nineteenth and early
twentieth century, more recently it has been thought that differences
disappear when corrections are made for body size and other variables.
In a decisive recent study of sex differences in brain size, C. Davison
Ankney reanalyzed well-controlled autopsy data from Cleveland, Ohio, in 1992.
Based on 1,261 individuals between the ages of twenty-five and eighty, he
found that, after correcting for body size, a 100-gram difference between men
and women and between European-Americans and African-Americans. Men averaged
1328 grams and women 1223 grams; European-Americans averaged 1320 grams and
African-Americans 1230 grams.
My own research confirmed Ankney's results in the use of a stratified
random sample of 6,325 United States Army personnel measured in 1988 for
fitting helmets and uniforms. After statistically adjusting for height,
weight, rank, and then sex or race, I found that men averaged 110 [cm.sup.3]
larger cranial capacities than women and Asian-Americans averaged about 60
[cm.sup.3] larger than African-Americans, with European-Americans
intermediate. In this study the sex difference was larger than the race
difference. Men averaged 1442 [cm.sup.3] and women 1332 [cm.sup.3] and
Asian-Americans, European-Americans, and African-Americans were, respectively,
1416, 1380, and 1359 [cm.sup.3]. Military rank differences were also found.
Officers averaged larger crania (1393 [cm.sup.3]) than enlisted personnel
(1375 [cm.sup.3]), even after correcting for body size.
Subsequently, I examined world-wide data from the International Labour
Office in Geneva. Head and body size figures were available from tens of
thousands of men and women sampled from twenty different regions - East and
West Africa, China and Japan, and European countries like Poland, France,
Portugal, and Spain. After correcting for body size, cranial capacity for men
averaged 160 [cm.sup.3] more than women, and Asians about 70 [cm.sup.3] more
than Africans, with Europeans intermediate.
These studies do not stand alone. Since 1980 several analyses of group
differences in brain size have been published, from autopsy and endocranial
measures as well as from those based on external head perimeter. Historically,
brain size data going back 100 years show Asians and Europeans with larger
brains than Africans. A small, but robust, relation has been firmly
established between mental ability and brain size. The correlation between
test scores and brain size estimated from magnetic resonance imaging tanges
from 0.35 to 0.47 with an average at about 0.40, as reported by Nancy
Andreasen and colleagues in 1993 in the American Journal of Psychiatry. This
represents a substantial increment over correlations teported since the turn
of the century between head perimeter and measures of intelligence which
average about 0.20.
Brain size-IQ relationships show up early in life. In the National
Collaborative Perinatal Project, 19,000 black infants had smaller head
perimeters at birth than 17,000 white infants, although black babies wete also
shorter in statute and lighter in weight. By age seven, catch-up growth
favored the black children in body size but not in head perimeter. Head
perimeter at birth correlated with IQ at age seven in both the black and the
white children.
Group differences in brain size mediate differences in mental ability.
With regard to gender differences in brain size, Ankney has pointed out a
paradox. Women have smaller brains than men but apparently have the same
intelligence test scores. Ankney resolved the problem by proposing that the
sex difference in btain size relates to those intellectual abilities at which
men excel. Men do better on various spatial tests and on tests of mathematical
reasoning.
As for race differences, reviews of the global literature show that people
of European ancestry living in North America, Europe, and Australia generally
obtain mean IQs of around 100. People of East Asian ancestry living in North
America and the Pacific Rim typically obtain slightly higher means, in the
range of 101 to 111. Africans from south of the Sahara, African-Americans, and
African-Caribbeans (including those living in Great Britain) obtain mean IQ
scores between 70 and 90.
However, the vexing question of whether IQ test scores are at all
revelatory about racial group differences in mental ability remains. At
bottom, the problem hinges on whether the tests are culture-bound. Although a
large body of technical work has disposed of this problem at the level of
psychometric expertise - the tests show similar patterns of internal
consistency and predictive validity for all groups, and the same differences
are to be found on relatively culture-free tests - doubts linger in many
quarters. Novel data on speed of decision making now show that the racial
group differences in mental ability are pervasive. Cross-cultural
investigations of reaction times have been carried out on nine-year-olds from
five countries. In these tasks, subjects must decide which of several lights
is on, or stands out from others, and move the hand to press a button. These
responses take less than a second to make but brighter children make them
significantly faster than less bright children. Richard Lynn found that
oriental children from Hong Kong and Japan are faster in decision time than
white children from Britain and Ireland, who in turn are faster than black
children from Africa. Using the same tests on slightly older samples, Arthur
Jensen has reported similar results in California.
These are not popular findings. They conflict with many deeply held
values. Let me then emphasize the importance of not exaggerating the findings.
There is enormous overlap in the distributions. The United States Army data
showed only an 8 percent difference separating the men and women in cranial
capacity and only a 4 percent difference separating Asian-Americans from
African-Americans. Also, in the Army data, black officers averaged a larger
cranial capacity (1369 [cm.sup.3]) than white enlisted personnel (1366
[cm.sup.3]). Clearly, it is highly problematic to generalize from a group
average to any particular individual.
As a result of carrying this research on brain size, I, like Cyril Burt,
had my reputation sullied. There was a call for my dismissal by the premier of
Ontario, a criminal investigation by the Ontario Provincial Police, a media
campaign against me, disruptions at the university, and an as yet unresolved
investigation by the Ontario Human Rights Commission. Stories of harassment
and intimidation could be told by others, among them Hans Eysenck in Great
Britain, Arthur Jensen at Berkeley, Tom Bouchard at Minnesota, Richard
Herrnstein at Harvard, Linda Gottfredson at Delaware, and Michael Levin at
City College of New York.
When Burt died in 1971, many people were gleeful. Equalitarian radicals
were prepared to believe that Burt had committed fraud, long before any
scandals were published. Charges of fraud were made all the time in personal
conversation about the work then being published by Jensen and Eysenck.
Charges of fraud are commonly raised in this research context. No one wanted
to believe that there was a genetic basis to racial differences in
intelligence.
It was the issue of race more than anything else that drove the attack on
Burt. At that time, Burt's data was the lynchpin of Jensen's and Eysenck's
work on race and it almost had to be discredited. Thus it was Leon Kamin in
America who was the first serious critic of Burt and then, of course, many
others entered the fray, including journalists and television producers.
Today, the campus radicals of earlier decades are the tenured radicals of
the 1990s. Some are chairmen of departments, deans of faculties, and
vice-presidents and presidents of universities. The 1960s mentality of peace,
love, and above all equality now constitutes a significant portion of the
intellectual establishment in the Western world. The equalitarian dogma is
more, not less, entrenched than ever befote. Yet, it is based on the
scientific hoax of the century.
It is interesting that the hoax about genetic equality has been
perpetuated for so long. Certainly one factor has been wishful thinking. We
would all like the world to be different than in fact it is. Few have been
eager to recognize the extent to which genes dictate what we are and what we
may become. The power of genes, however, will become progressively harder to
deny as the Human Genome Project nears completion. Many prefer not to know,
because ignorance allows hope while knowledge can destroy it. The best way to
predict your IQ is to average the IQ of your biological parents. That
prediction holds regardless of whether you were raised totally separated from
your biological parents. Ultimately we will be able to predict IQ scores by
taking a single cell from an embryo.
For some, work on the genetics of intelligence, and racial differences
therein, challenges the Enlightenment assumption that knowledge is always
better than ignorance. But scholars have accepted that the earth is not the
center of the universe, and that man's closest living relatives are the
chimpanzees. We can yet affirm our common humanity by accepting our
differences. The disparagement of Cyril Burt is the most extraordinary case of
counterfeit charges in the history of academic psychology, if not all of
science.
READINGS SUGGESTED BY THE AUTHOR:
C. Davison Ankney. Sex Differences in Relative Brain Size: The Mismeasure
of Woman, Too?" Intelligence, 16 (1992), 329-336.
Ronald Fietcher. Science, Ideology, and the Media. New Brunswick, N. J.:
Transaction Publishers, 1991.
Arthur R. Jensen. "Scientific Fraud or False Accusations? Tbe Case of
Cyril Burt.- In D. J. Miller and M. Hersen (eds.) Research Fraud in the
Behavioral and Biomedical Sciences. New York: John Wiley, 1992.
Richard Lynn. "Race Differences in Intelligence: A Global Perspective.
Mankind Quarterly, 31 (1991), 255-296.
Roger Pearson. Race, Intelligence, and Bias in Academe. Washington, D.C.:
Scott-townsend, 1991.
J. Philippe Rushton. "Cranial Capacity Related to Sex, Rank and Race in a
Stratified Sample of 6,325 U.S. Military Personnel." Intelligence, 16 (1992),
401-413.
Mark Snyderman and Stanley Rothman. The IQ Controversy, the Media, and
Public Policy. New Brunswick, N. J.: Transaction Publishers, 1988.
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone