Open Letters
THE ORION PARTY
The Prometheus League
- Humanity Needs A World Government PDF
- Cosmos Theology Essay PDF
- Cosmos Theology Booklet PDF
- Europe Destiny Essays PDF
- Historical Parallels PDF
- Christianity Examined PDF
News Blogs
Euvolution
- Home Page
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
Reviewed by Leila Zenderland
Science, May 18, 1990 v248 n4957 p884(3)
LEILA ZENDERLAND Department of American Studies, California State
University, Fullerton, CA 92634
Robert Joynson The Burt Affair
On 24 October 1976, the London Sunday Times published a front-page story
headed "Crucial data was faked by eminent psychologist." "The most sensational
charge of scientific fraud this century," it began, "is being levelled against
the late Sir Cyril Burt. . . . Leading scientists are convinced that Burt
published false data and invented crucial facts to support his controversial
theory that intelligence is largely inherited." The ensuing scandal forms the
subject of Robert Joynson's book. Joynson's research is likely to inspire at
least one more round in this controversy, for he argues that Burt has been
unjustly maligned.
Even in a field as prone to public controversy as intelligence testing,
the Burt affair forms an exceptionally dramatic and disturbing episode. Burt,
a brilliant mathematician, author of Factors of the Mind (1940), and editor of
the British Journal of Statistical Psychology, had been a pioneer in
educational psychology. His studies of intelligence also contained data on the
largest number of identical twins raised apart--key evidence in debates
between hereditarians and environmentalists. Knighted in 1946, Burt was
Britain's most honored psychologist during his lifetime. As "long as
psychology remains a subject of scientific inquiry," psychologist Leslie
Hearnshaw noted in his eulogy following Burt's death in 1971, "he will live in
its halls of fame" (p. 27).
By 1972, however, environmentalist Leon Kamin had begun to raise serious
questions about Burt's science in answering hereditarian Arthur Jensen. Burt's
articles on intelligence testing, Kamin observed, were suspiciously lacking in
basic information, such as place, time, and type of test administered, and
were filled with highly improbable coincidences, such as correlations that
remained exactly the same--to three decimal places--even when sample sizes had
more than doubled. By 1974, Kamin and Jensen had reached rare agreement:
Burt's "correlations are useless for hypothesis testing," wrote Jensen; his
numbers "are simply not worthy of our scientific attention," charged Kamin (p.
162).
Charging fraud was another matter. Kamin's writing, however, had
interested Times reporter Oliver Gillie, who began searching for Burt's
research assistants, Margaret Howard and Jane Conway. Finding no evidence of
their existence, past or present, and being informed that these were probably
pseudonyms invented by Burt, Gillie broke the story.
Caught in the ensuing crossfire was Hearnshaw, who at the time was working
on a biography commissioned by Burt's sister. Hearnshaw agreed to examine the
new charges, and when published in 1979 his study, Cyril Burt, Psychologist,
described a gifted scientist whose early research had probably been genuine,
but whose data, largely destroyed by wartime bombings, had probably been
partly fabricated in postwar writings. Burt's personal and published papers
since the 1940s, Hearnshaw concluded, suggested a pattern of deliberate deceit
in claims about his role in his field's history, the quantity of new data
collected, and the number of assistants helping him. In fact, of 40 different
"authors" who published material in the journal Burt edited, over half may
have been Burt himself, writing under pseudonyms. In light of Hearnshaw's
findings, the British Psychological Society declared Burt guilty of fraud.
Joynson now proposes that the Burt investigation be reopened. The
Society's actions, he argues, were premature, for the charges remain to be
proven. Burt, he maintains, will be exonerated.
Joynson argues his case like a wily defense lawyer. Burt must be presumed
innocent, he insists, until proven guilty beyond a shadow of a doubt. And the
burden of proof must lie with the prosecution. Moreover, the standards of
admissible evidence must be narrowed. Hearsay must be disallowed. Memories by
contemporaries should be admitted only when accompanied by written
documentation from the period in question. And all written evidence should be
unambiguous. The defense has an easier task: it must prove only that one or
more explanations besides fraud are possible.
Joynson's explanations for the many charges against Burt range from the
plausible to the incredible. Burt, he argues, may simply have used outdated
methods, or exercised poor judgment. Or he may have fallen victim to political
enemies, jealous colleagues, or even a conspiracy of gossip-mongerers. (At one
point Joynson shows that several of those who now believe Burt guilty,
including hereditarian Hans Eysenck, had once worked at Maudsley Hospital,
where they may have heard unsubstantiated rumors about Burt's
untrustworthiness.)
In this case, the best defense is a good offense. Joynson's main target is
Hearnshaw's biography, which he blames for turning the tide against Burt and
contends is full of errors and unproven assumptions. He focuses on four of
Hearnshaw's main charges: that Burt changed the historical record; that his
kinship studies, with their suspicious correlations and missing assistants,
suggest fraud; that he lied about the sources for his final papers; and that
he suffered from mental illness.
Joynson spends much time examining Burt's historical claims. Did Burt, as
Hearnshaw contends, try to diminish Charles Spearman's contributions to factor
analysis to promote his own? Hearnshaw's book documents Burt's pattern of
distorting evidence to exaggerate his own priority; Joynson argues with him,
footnote by footnote. Burt may have become "less deferential" toward Spearman,
Joynson concedes, but this is hardly an indictable offense.
The kinship studies present a more central challenge, and here Joynson is
most unconvincing. Critics have found Burt's published claims difficult to
challenge precisely because he is so vague in supplying details. Nonetheless,
of 64 correlation coefficients reportedly calculated on new samples in Burt's
1966 study, as many as 30 are overtly suspicious, for they repeat figures
found in earlier articles. The most famous of these--an intelligence
correlation of .771 reported in 1955 for 21 pairs of identical twins raised
apart, and in 1966 for 53 pairs--Joynson believes may have been "a genuine
coincidence" (p. 155). As for the others, Joynson proposes, Burt may simply
have inserted old figures if he had no new data, without realizing the need to
mention that these came from different samples. In any case, Joynson accuses
critics of paying too much attention to Burt's suspicious figures and too
little to the 34 that are new. After all, Joynson reasons, if repeated
coefficients suggest fraud, then "by the same logic we must also now argue
that the appearance of a new coefficient suggests that the data are genuine"
(pp. 156-157). Such logic speaks for itself.
In considering Burt's "missing assistants," Joynson endorses an
explanation offered by Burt's former colleague Charlotte Banks. Like
Hearnshaw, Joynson believes that Howard and Conway may have been volunteer
social workers whom Burt met before the war. Both also agree that Burt
probably conducted no major new twin studies after 1950. The data in Burt's
later papers, Joynson proposes, were prewar materials--materials misplaced in
Burt's many moves, gradually rediscovered, and then published. This was
possible, he argues, for Burt's secretary was, in Banks's words, "very
accurate but couldn't file for toffee, and was very sensitive to criticism."
"I am sure he would have promised her not to say the material had been lost,"
writes Banks (p. 180). To Joynson, this story suggests "possible answers"
explaining postwar publications credited to Burt's prewar assistants.
Joynson's most serious charge against Hearnshaw concerns Burt's final
papers. In 1969, Burt published test results reportedly gathered between 1914
and 1965 showing school performance declining. Hearnshaw quotes an interview
that supposedly appeared in The Guardian in which Burt claimed that his tests
had been given regularly to hundreds of schoolchildren--a claim Hearnshaw then
proves to be a lie. Joynson, however, has been unable to locate any such
interview. Such a charge must be answered. Nonetheless, even if found, Joynson
argues, the interview will not prove decisive, since newspaper accounts are
notoriously unreliable.
Joynson's final strategy is to discredit Hearnshaw's explanations for
Burt's behavior--mental illness and childhood influences. There is no
independent evidence, Joynson contends, that Burt suffered any mental
instability. Moreover, whatever survival instincts Burt manifested had
probably been learned not from the "'gamin' subculture" that Hearnshaw claims
he had known as a child living near the slums but from academic life, which
"revolves around backbiting, innuendo, second-hand gossip, and abuse of
confidence" (p. 256).
Such a defense leaves a strange effect. Joynson may believe that he has
exonerated his client; Burt, however, hardly leaves this courtroom with his
reputation intact. Ironically, Joynson's "innocent" Burt emerges as an even
less likable character than Hearnshaw's "guilty" Burt. Such a verdict,
however, is acceptable to Joynson; Burt's methods may have been less than
admirable, he argues, but they were short of criminal.
Joynson's arguments are sure to invoke detailed rejoinders from those now
called "anti-Burters." Of more concern than Burt's posthumous reputation,
however, is the broader question of standards of evidence, both scientific and
historical, raised here. Joynson's research contains nothing to challenge the
current consensus that, as scientific evidence, Burt's data are unacceptable.
Moreover, even if one believed Burt innocent of conscious wrongdoing, the fact
that such data were used in debates over educational policies and went
unchallenged until the 1970s would still be scandalous.
Historians, however, can rarely invoke such strict standards in admitting
evidence. Unfortunately, like Hearnshaw, they must often draw their
conclusions from incomplete records, ambiguous writings, and the memories of
contemporaries--the same materials Joynson uses to construct his alternative
explanations. Burt may never have received his day in court; his place in
history, however, must now be judged by the work he left behind.
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone