Open Letters
THE ORION PARTY
The Prometheus League
- Humanity Needs A World Government PDF
- Cosmos Theology Essay PDF
- Cosmos Theology Booklet PDF
- Europe Destiny Essays PDF
- Historical Parallels PDF
- Christianity Examined PDF
News Blogs
Euvolution
- Home Page
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Library of Eugenics
- Genetic Revolution News
- Science
- Philosophy
- Politics
- Nationalism
- Cosmic Heaven
- Eugenics
- Future Art Gallery
- NeoEugenics
- Contact Us
- About the Website
- Site Map
Transhumanism News
Partners
The Role of Cognition in Evolutionary Theory
By F. J. Irsigler, Paardekraal Hospital, Krugersdorp, Transvaal Mankind
Quarterly, 33, 06-01-1993, pp 371.
Lynn (The Mankind Quarterly, XXXII, 1-2, 1991: 116) argues that brain size
is positively correlated with intelligence in man, and that races show
consistent differences in both brain size and intelligence. Intelligence is
understood as the ability to solve cognitive problems. It consists of a
conscious (rational) and an unconscious (ratiomorphic) component. The first
originates from education, the second from the process of phylogenetic
learning (a posteriori) but at the same time is an expectation (a priori) for
the ontogenetic gain of knowledge (R. Riedl et al. in Evolution and Cognition
2:58, 1992).
I. The Basic Relation
Evolution is based on the functional relation between ontogeny and
phylogeny (Schindewolf, Gould). According to the French molecular biologist F.
Jacob, the relation allows to convert a series of organizations in space into
an isomorphic series of transformations in time, called "La logique du vivant"
(Jacob 1970: 162, 318). In structural terms, the relation is equivalent to
autonomous morphogenesis (Monod 1972:26), that is, interpenetration (L.
Edinger 1909) or interdigitation (H. Braak 1980) of the phylogenetically old
and the new cortices, the allocortex (sensu lato) and the neo-or isocortex. In
functional terms, the basic relation refers to the conflict between organism
and environment in which the organism "always calls the tune (Jacob: 185).
This is due to a cognitive state known as "conscious" or "the conscious self"
. It is innate in each species (Sperry 1983: 95) and at the base of the
autonomy or self-determination characterizing the living belongs in their
behavior (Monod: 79). The latter is related to a specific kind of information
from the outside world but does not depend on changes in the environment. This
information is called "semantic" (Brillouin) or "teleonomic" (Monod).
Professor M. Eigen, the Nobel laureate from the Max-Planck-Institutfur
biophysikalische Chemie in Gottingen, calls it "Information of selective value
with respect to reproduction" (Eigen 1971:517). It provides the "perceived
meaning" (Stent) of signs and symbols, also called "the emotional truth"
(Cattell). Both are species-typical raising the question how the new
evolutionary types or "morphs" originate.
II. Evolution by Law
New species, or subspecific types arise by heteromorph speciation. This
means sudden events called "heterochronic", occurring during individual
ontogenies and disrupting Haekel's repetition of phylogeny in ontogeny. This
is the tenet of the emerging school holding "Evolution by Law" or
"Nomogenesis" from the Greek nomos = law (L. Berg 1922/1969, J.C. Willis 1940,
C.P. Groves 1989). Heterochronic "jumps" are explained by G.R. de Beer who
states in his "Embryology and Evolution" (1930:108): "New characters apply at
all stages of ontogeny, and by heterochony they may be retarded or
accelerated, so as to appear later or earlier in subsequent ontogenies". This
makes phylogeny a function of ontogeny (A.N. Sewertzoff 1931: 365) and leads
to evolutionary progress by phylogenetical branching or cladogenesis (B.
Rensch 1971: 122). It is exemplified by the split in the hominid lineage
during the Late Pliocene (Tobias 1985:135) and the sudden transposition at the
time of emergence of the modern and Neanderthal grade (Groves: 60 ff, 304,
317).
Retarded ontogeny results in retention of ancestral "juvenile characters" ,
formerly called foetalization, now known as "paedo-morphosis/neoteny" .
It is the retention of their juvenile bodily traits into adulthood which
makes the South African Kalahari Bushmen or San people a distinct ethnic or
racial entity (Tobias 1978: 5). The ancestors of the San, living in richly
endowed areas, looked exactly like the present-day desert dwellers. This shows
that their paedomorphic traits are not, as formerly thought, an effect of
adaptation to the arid environment. The homogenetic school considers
adaptation to be a conservative rather than progressive factor in evolution.
It holds that our species is geologically young and demic (stenotopic) right
from the start.
III. Space-time (Dissipative) Structures
Professor I. Prigogine, of the Free University in Brusselles, has developed
a thermo-dynamical theory of evolution which he calls a "Dialogue with
Nature". The theory rests on two premises developed in Prigogine's Nobel
Lecture in Chemistry (1977). (1) The thermodynamical a state "far from
equilibrium" which characterizes the living beings, leads, through a series of
instability, to a new "order through fluctuations" .(2) From the instabilities
result, through exchange of matter and energy (information) with the outside
world, in space-time structures, called "dissipative" (Prigogine 1977: 2.1,
4.5). The exchange is both necessary and irreversible. Dissipativity thus
requires the isomorphism of the basic relation (Section I) to be replaced by
heteromorphism leading to heteromorph speciation (Section II). The
fluctuations play a decisive role in the evolutionary progress. They amplify
some initially small variations into behavior related to the specific "Umwelt"
to which the living beings belong. This leads Prigogine to the concept of
multiple times (Prigogine et Stengers: 13, 431). The group-specific time is
represented in the new "time evolution operator" with its "eigen values". It
expresses the condition of dissipativity as a (Lyapounof) function > O
(Progogine 1977: 7.6) Dissapativity breaks the time-symmetry of the "Classics"
and results, through successive bifurcations, to phylogenetical branching
(cladogenesis).
There were earlier attempts to conceive homogenization in terms of a series
of deviation-amplifying processes. Professor P. V. Tobias, University of the
Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, refers in his James Arthur lecture on the
Evolution of the Human Brain (1969) to Maruyama (1963) and to Holloway (1967)
arguing that an initial "kick or push" , such as tool-using in Late Pliocene
pongids, may have set in motion a positive feedback" resulting in a
cladogenetic split (Tobias 1971:144 ff).
The crucial point is the autonomy of the heterochronic (space-time)
transformations in which fluctuations, structure, and function are
reciprocally interrelated Prigogine 1977: 4.5).
Dissipative structures mediating the contact between organism and the
outside world, are present in animals ranging from reptiles to man. They are
called "the reptilian-type and palaeo-mammalian-typebrains" (Maclean 1978:
42). They comprise the olfacto-striate complex and satellite grisea, the
so-called "periventricular brain" involved in hormonal feedback regulation
(Nieuwenhuys 1985:179 ff), Laborit's "information circulante". It coincides
largely, but not entirely, with the limbic or palaeo-mammalian system which
undergoes, in the ascending primates, a definite progression in size and
differentiation culminating in man (Stephan and Andy 1970, Stephan 1975). The
interdigitation between the phylogenetically old and the new cortices (Section
I) results from the migratory processes (Schepers 1948:167) characterizing the
early stages of cortex evolution in mammals, that is hemispheric rotation
around the lateral (sylvian) fossa, and infolding of the allocortex forming a
zone of transition where allocortical and isocortical layers are intimately
indented (Braak: 38).
If the connecting pathways between the reptilian and palaeo-mammalian
formations are destroyed in monkeys, the species-typical, simian behavior
disappears. Interpreting these experimental findings in the light of clinical
evidence one might say that these structures "provide the avenues to the basic
personality" (Maclean:49).
Clinical evidence is available. In the type of presenile dementia first
described by A. Pick in 1898 and bearing his name, "the avenues or connecting
regions of the olfactory with the limbic system" are the initial and selective
targets of the cortical atrophy in the early stages of Pick's disease. It
eventually destroys the human character and conduct ("Kern des Menschseins").
This and the fatal outcome are due to the progressive destruction of the
orbito-frontotemporal cortices (Spatz' "basale Rinde") including the
allocortical regio entorhinalis and Brodmann's "insula ventralis or
olfactoria" (Stephan 1975, Irsigler 1989). (Spatz 1937, 1955, 1965, Luers and
Spatz 1957, Kahle 1969, Jacob 1979).
Summarizing. problems arising from space-time contact between organism and
environment require structures not related to cognition, but controlled by
emotions or "drives". These are components of the "conscious self" ranging
from animal consciousness to the self-conscious mind of the human species
(Popper and Eccles). The "affective unconscious and the cognitive unconscious"
of the Piagetian school (1976) is innate in each species and develops onto-
and phylo-genetically at different rates, that is, by heterochronic
cytogenesis and mylogenesis (Spatz and coworkers). It follows that the
Prigoginean "space-time structures" must include al, cortex sensu lato,
represented in mammals by the "periventricular or
reptilian-palaeo-mammalian"(cholinergic) brains (MacLean, Niewenhuys). It is
thus the subcortex and its connecting path with "basal neocortex" the
subcortex and its connecting school, not the surface "association" areas which
are at the base of the "distributed" cerebral functioning (Pribram). In other
words: the correlation between brain size and function (including
intelligence) is one of structural (allo-isocortical) interpenetration and
functional reciprocity, not one of sheer quantity in the sense of Jacob's
"isomorphism" . The determining factor in both phylogeny and ontogeny is group
specificity. In man, this means innate individuality "that effectively rules
out environment, experience, or known theory of child development or
nurturance" (Sperry 1983: 56).
The heterochronic "jumps" (Phasensprunge, Eigen 1988) characterizing the
living beings are related to species-typical information with respect to
reproduction. These sudden events replace the time parameter of causation with
the quasi-timeless parameter of implication (Hormann, Lestienne) or inductive
reasoning (Induktion, Riedl 1980). Heteromorph speciation defines the
participation of the species in their own evolution.
Conclusion
In man, brain size is determined by the mass of the neo-cortex. This has
led to the idea that the higher cortical functions, including consciousness,
are neocortical in origin and character, resulting from the cognitive
evolution starting "from scratch" after the break- up of instinct (Piaget
1976). However, consciousness, or the conscious self, is innate in each
species, amounting in man to "innate individuality" (Sperry 1983). It is a
function of a basic relation (A.N. Whitehead 1949), that is, of the
group-specific space-time converting "dissipative" structures present in the
brain since the Jurassic mammalian-like reptiles including man. Both the
conscious and the unconscious components of cognition are related to the
"Umwelt" to which living beings belong, (this is K. Lorenz' "evolutionary
epistemology" qualified by I. Prigogine' s "irreversible thermodynamics"). It
is the species-typical autonomy characterizing the cognitive (intellectual and
emotional dimensions of the human personality) that is responsible for the
individual and group differences to which Lynn refers between the sexes and
the races.
REFERENCES
De Beer, G. R.
1930 Embryology and Evolution, Oxford University Press
Berg. L.
1969 Nomogenesis or evolution determined by (trans. J.N. Rostovsov). MIT
Press, Cambridge, Mass.
Braak, H.
1980 Architectonics of the Human Telencephalon. Springer, Berlin.
Brillouin, L.
1971 Science and Information Theory. Academic Press, New York.
Cattell, R. B.
1972 A New Morality from Science: Beyondism. Pergamon Press, New York.
Edinger, L.
1909 Einfuhrung in die Lehre vom Bau und Verrichtungen des Nervensystems.
F.C.Wogel, Leipzig.
Eigen, M.
1971 Self Organization of Matter and the Evolution of Biological
Macromolecules. Die Naturwissenschaften, 59:465-523
1998 Biologische Selbstorganisation: Eine Abfolge von Phasen sprungen. In
K. Hierholzer and H.-G. Wittmann (eds) Phasen-srunge und Steigkeit in der
naturlichen und kukturellen Welt. Stuttgart, Wissenschaftliche
Verlagsgesellschaft: 113-147.
Irsigler, F. J.
1971 Brain Size, Intelligence and Race - Fact and fiction. A Critique of an
Academic Lecture. Mankind Quarterly XII/1:20-28.
1989 Language origin and the Island of Reil. In J. Wind, E.G. Pulleyblank,
E. de Grolier and B.H. Bichaljian (eds) Studies in Language Origins. J.
Benjamin Publishing Company, Amsterdam: 233-256.
Jacob, F.
1970 La logique du vivant. Gallimard, Paros.
Jakob, H.
1979 Die Picksche Krankheit. Springer, Berlin
Jenson, A. R.
1973 Educability and group Differences, Methuen, London.
Kahle, W.
1969 Die Entwicklung der menschlichen Grosshirnisphare, Springer, Berlin.
Laborit, H.
1974 Des betes et des hommes. Agresologie 15/2:93-107.
Luers, TH. und H. Spatz
1957 Picksche Krankheit. In W. Scholz (ed) Hanbuch der spezielen
pathologischen Anatomie und Histologie. Band XIII/Nervensystem: 614-715.
Lynn, R.
1991 The Evolution of Racial Differences in Intelligence. The Mankind
Quarterly XXXII, 1-2: 99-121.
Lorenz, K.
1965 Revolution and Modication of Behavior. The University of Chicago
Press, Chicago Press, Chicago Press, Chicago
Maclean, P. D.
1978 The Evolution of Three Mentalities. In S.L. Wasburn and E.R. McCown
(eds) Human Evolution. The Benjamin/Cummings Publishing Company, Menlo Park
(California): 33-57.
Monod, J.
1972 Chance and Necessity. Collins, London.
Nieuwenhuys, R.
1985 Chemoarchitecture of the Brain. Springer, Berlin.
Piaget, J.
1976 The Affective Unconcious and the Cognitive Unconscious. In B. Inhelder
and H.H. Chapman (eds.) Piaget and His School, 63-71.
Prigogine I.
1977 Time, Structure and Fluctuations. Nobel Lecture in Chemistry.
Prigogine, I. and I. Stengers
1979 La nouvelle alliance. Gallimard, Paris.
Popper, K. R. and J. C. Eccles
1977 The Self and Its Brain. Springer International, Berlin.
Rensch, B.
1971 Biophilosophy. Columbia University Press, New York.
Riedl, R.
1980 Biologie der Erkenntnis. Paul Parey, Berlin/Hamburg.
Riedl, R., G. Ackermann and L. Huber
1992 A ratiomorphic problem solving strategy. Evolution and Cognition
2:23-61.
Schepers, G. W. H.
1948 Evolution of the Forebrain. Maskew, Cape Town.
Schindewolf O. H.
1950 Der Zeitfaktor in Geologie und Palaontologie. Schweizerbart,
Stuttgart.
Schrodinger, E.
1967 What is Life? Mind and Matter. Cambridge University Press
Sewertzoff, A. N.
1931 Morphologishe Gesetzmassigkeiten der Evolution. Fischer, Jena.
Spatz, H.
1937 Uber die Bedeutung der basalen Rinde auf Grund von Beobachtungen bei
Pickscher Zeitscrift fur die Gesamte Nerologie und Psychiatrie 158; 208-232.
1955 Die Evolution Menschenhirns und ihre Bedeutung fur die Sonderstellung
des Menschen. GiessenerHochschulgessellschaft24:58-74.
1966 Gehirnentwicklung (Introversion-Promination)und Endocranialausguss. In
R. Hassler und H.Stephan (Eds) Evolution of the forebrain: 136- 152. Thieme,
Stuttgart.
Sperry, R.
1983 Science and Moral Priority. Blackwell, Oxford.
Stent, G.
1975 Limits to the Scientific Understanding of Man. Science 189: 1052-
1057. 1952-1057.
Stephan H.
1975 Allocortex. In W. Bargmann (ed) Handbuch der mikroskopischen Anatomie
des Mensachen IV/9:1-988 Anatomie des Menschen IV/9:1-988.
Stephan, H. and O. J. Andy
1970 The allocortex in primates. In C.R. Noback and W. Montagna (eds) The
primate brain. Advances in primatology 1:109-135. Appleton-Century Crofts.
Tobias, P. V.
1971 The brain in hominid evolution. Columbia University Press.
1978 The Bushmen, San Huntersand Herders. Humansand Rousseau, Cape Town.
1985 Punctuational and Phyletic Evolution in the Hominids. In E. S. Vrba
(ed) Species and Speciation. Transvaal Museum Monograph 4:131- 141, Pretoria.
Whitehead, A. N.
1949 Wissenschaft und Moderne Welt. Mortgarten Verlag Conzett und Huber,
Zurich.
Willis, J. C.
1940 The Course of Evolution. Cambridge University Press.
Transtopia
- Main
- Pierre Teilhard De Chardin
- Introduction
- Principles
- Symbolism
- FAQ
- Transhumanism
- Cryonics
- Island Project
- PC-Free Zone